@article{lee_neupert_2024, title={The effect of control beliefs on the relationship between daily stressors and subjective age in younger adults}, url={https://doi.org/10.1002/mhs2.56}, DOI={10.1002/mhs2.56}, abstractNote={Abstract}, journal={Mental Health Science}, author={Lee, Sofia E. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2024}, month={Mar} } @article{early_reynolds_neupert_2024, title={Traumatic losses permeate daily emotional experiences: roles of daily uplifts and subjective age}, volume={1}, ISSN={["1758-0854"]}, url={https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12530}, DOI={10.1111/aphw.12530}, abstractNote={Abstract}, journal={APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY-HEALTH AND WELL BEING}, author={Early, Alexandra S. and Reynolds, Caitlin M. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2024}, month={Jan} } @article{pearman_hughes_coblenz_smith_neupert_2023, title={A Precautionary Tale: Individual Decision Making in the Time of COVID-19}, volume={20}, ISSN={1660-4601}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20054597}, DOI={10.3390/ijerph20054597}, abstractNote={Precaution taking is an important part of managing COVID-19 and has been since the start of the pandemic. Guided by the Health Belief Model, two studies conducted during the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic aimed to identify possible individual difference predictors of precautionary actions. Study 1 was an online, cross-sectional study using 763 adults aged 20–79 years old. Study 2, a 30-day daily diary study, examined daily precautions in 261 persons over the age of 55 years old. Study 1 and Study 2 indicated that COVID-19 knowledge predicted precautionary behaviors. Multilevel models from Study 2 indicated that daily increases in in-person interactions and leaving home were associated with decreases in precautions, but increases in disruption to routine were associated with increases in precautions. In both studies, including concurrent and lagged models in Study 2, significant interactions between information seeking and perceived risk suggested higher information seeking was related to higher precautions for those who consider themselves low risk. Findings highlight the burden of daily precautions and potentially modifiable factors of engagement in precautions.}, number={5}, journal={International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health}, publisher={MDPI AG}, author={Pearman, Ann and Hughes, MacKenzie L. and Coblenz, Clara W. and Smith, Emily L. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2023}, month={Mar}, pages={4597} } @article{dieffenderfer_brewer_noonan_smith_eichenlaub_haley_jacks_lobaton_neupert_hess_et al._2023, title={A Wearable System for Continuous Monitoring and Assessment of Speech, Gait, and Cognitive Decline for Early Diagnosis of ADRD}, ISSN={["1558-4615"]}, DOI={10.1109/EMBC40787.2023.10339986}, abstractNote={Early detection of cognitive decline is essential to study mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease in order to develop targeted interventions and prevent or stop the progression of dementia. This requires continuous and longitudinal assessment and tracking of the related physiological and behavioral changes during daily life. In this paper, we present a low cost and low power wearable system custom designed to track the trends in speech, gait, and cognitive stress while also considering the important human factor needs such as privacy and compliance. In the form factors of a wristband and waist-patch, this multimodal, multi-sensor system measures inertial signals, sound, heart rate, electrodermal activity and pulse transit time. A total power consumption of 2.6 mW without any duty cycling allows for more than 3 weeks of run time between charges when 1500 mAh batteries are used.Clinical Relevance— Much earlier detection of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias may be possible by continuous monitoring of physiological and behavioral state using application specific wearable sensors during the activities of daily life.}, journal={2023 45TH ANNUAL INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE OF THE IEEE ENGINEERING IN MEDICINE & BIOLOGY SOCIETY, EMBC}, author={Dieffenderfer, James and Brewer, Alec and Noonan, Maxwell A. and Smith, Madeline and Eichenlaub, Emily and Haley, Katarina L. and Jacks, Adam and Lobaton, Edgar and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Hess, Thomas M. and et al.}, year={2023} } @article{hughes_neupert_smith_coblenz_macy_pearman_2023, title={COVID‐19 vaccine hesitancy: The synergistic effect of anxiety and proactive coping}, volume={2}, ISSN={2769-2450 2769-2450}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/puh2.70}, DOI={10.1002/puh2.70}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={1}, journal={Public Health Challenges}, publisher={Wiley}, author={Hughes, MacKenzie L. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Smith, Emily L. and Coblenz, Clara W. and Macy, Samuel G. and Pearman, Ann}, year={2023}, month={Mar} } @article{johnson_neupert_2023, title={Combatting Election Stress: Anticipatory Coping and Daily Self-Reported Physical Health}, volume={3}, ISSN={0033-2941 1558-691X}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00332941231165444}, DOI={10.1177/00332941231165444}, abstractNote={ This study’s purpose is to evaluate the effectiveness of anticipatory coping as a possible mechanism to reduce daily self-reported physical health consequences of forecasting election-related stress. Elections are often stressful for people. However, the research on possible physical consequences of anticipated election-related stress and ways to counter its impact is limited. We used data from the 2018 U.S. Midterm Election Stress Coping and Prevention Every Day (ESCAPED) study, which provided daily diary responses from 140 participants across the U.S. and American Samoa, ages 19–86. Participants were recruited using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (mTurk) and were asked to fill out an online survey each day for 22 days before, the day of, and 7 days after the 2018 U.S. midterm election, which fell on November 6, 2018. Questions involved forecasting election-related stress, four forms of anticipatory coping, and daily self-reported physical health. Results from multilevel models suggested that on days when participants experienced an increase in their forecasting of election-related stressors and also experienced a decrease in their anticipatory coping related to problem analysis, their daily self-reported physical health decreased. These results highlight the role that future-oriented tactics could play in managing the physical consequences of election-related stress. }, journal={Psychological Reports}, publisher={SAGE Publications}, author={Johnson, Brittany K. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2023}, month={Mar}, pages={003329412311654} } @article{zhang_neupert_2023, title={Control Beliefs Mediate the Within-Person Relationship Between Everyday Activity Volition and Daily Subjective Age}, volume={8}, ISSN={["1758-5368"]}, url={https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbad125}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbad125}, abstractNote={Abstract}, journal={JOURNALS OF GERONTOLOGY SERIES B-PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES AND SOCIAL SCIENCES}, author={Zhang, Shenghao and Neupert, Shevaun}, editor={Barber, SarahEditor}, year={2023}, month={Aug} } @article{dunsmore_neupert_2023, title={Coping With 'Scanxiety': Within-Person Processes in Lung Cancer}, volume={3}, ISSN={["1558-691X"]}, url={https://doi.org/10.1177/00332941231164336}, DOI={10.1177/00332941231164336}, abstractNote={ Background: Patients with early-stage lung cancer undergo potentially curative therapy, and continue to undergo regularly scheduled CT scans to determine if cancer has reappeared, spread, or stayed the same. This process can be fraught with anxiety, coined ‘Scanxiety’. The present study examined how coping and scan-related anxiety fluctuate within-person before one’s scan. Method: Twenty five individuals with lung cancer who had received curative intent treatment (M age = 62.33, [SD = 8.10], 96% women, 80% white) participated in the study, which had two parts. First, participants provided information about proactive coping and scan-related anxiety every 30 days. Next, a daily diary study was implemented for 7 consecutive days before their CT scan, as well as the day of their CT scan, where participants reported on their daily anticipatory coping and scan-related anxiety. The 25 participants provided 59 monthly and 146 daily surveys for analysis. Results: Multilevel models revealed significant main effects of monthly proactive coping on monthly scan-related anxiety, as well as daily anticipatory coping on daily scan-related anxiety. On months when participants decreased their use of proactive coping, they also reported decreases in scan-related anxiety for that month. On days when participants reported decreases in outcome fantasy and stagnant deliberation, they reported decreases in scan-related anxiety for that day. Finally, a significant interaction was found such that on days when middle-aged adults reported increases in problem analysis, they also reported increases in scan-related anxiety for that day. Conclusion: These findings are the first to characterize how participants’ coping and scanxiety fluctuate in the months and days prior to their CT scans. Results indicated that focusing on the present may be more beneficial in reducing scan-related anxiety rather than thinking about the future. Future work should implement strategies to reduce scanxiety by focusing on the present among those with lung cancer. }, journal={PSYCHOLOGICAL REPORTS}, author={Dunsmore, Victoria J. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2023}, month={Mar} } @article{digirolamo_neupert_isaacowitz_2023, title={Emotion Regulation Convoys: Individual and Age Differences in the Hierarchical Configuration of Emotion Regulation Behaviors in Everyday Life}, volume={12}, ISSN={["2662-205X"]}, url={https://doi.org/10.1007/s42761-023-00228-8}, DOI={10.1007/s42761-023-00228-8}, abstractNote={Abstract}, journal={AFFECTIVE SCIENCE}, author={DiGirolamo, Marissa A. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Isaacowitz, Derek M.}, year={2023}, month={Dec} } @article{graham_neupert_2023, title={Mindfulness, inductive reasoning, and awareness of age‐related changes: A daily diary study}, url={https://doi.org/10.1002/mhs2.32}, DOI={10.1002/mhs2.32}, abstractNote={Abstract}, journal={Mental Health Science}, author={Graham, Lyndsey N. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2023}, month={Dec} } @article{neupert_smith_schriefer_2022, title={A Coordinated Analysis of Physical Reactivity to Daily Stressors: Age and Proactive Coping Matter}, volume={4}, ISSN={2571-9394}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/forecast4040054}, DOI={10.3390/forecast4040054}, abstractNote={Proactive coping involves efforts to prepare for future stressors and may have implications for physical responses to stress. We examined age differences in physical reactivity to daily stressors moderated by proactive coping in a coordinated analysis across two separate daily diary studies. Study 1 included data from 116 older (age range 60–90) and 107 younger (age range 18–36) adults on daily stressors and physical health symptoms for 8 consecutive days. Study 2 included data from 140 adults (age range 19–86) on daily stressors and self-rated physical health for 29 consecutive days. Participants in both studies reported on their proactive coping on the first day of the study. Physical reactivity was indexed via lagged multilevel models as increases in daily physical symptoms in Study 1 and decreases in daily physical health in Study 2 with corresponding increases in daily stressors. Results indicated that in both studies, younger adults with low proactive coping were more physically reactive to daily stressors compared to younger adults with high proactive coping. Proactive coping was associated with reduced physical reactivity to daily stressors among younger adults, consistent with the characterization of a high degree of control and ample opportunities at earlier phases of adulthood which are critical for accumulating resources to proactively cope.}, number={4}, journal={Forecasting}, publisher={MDPI AG}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Smith, Emily L. and Schriefer, Margaret L.}, year={2022}, month={Nov}, pages={1004–1018} } @article{cobler_smith_neupert_2022, title={Age and socioeconomic differences in coping with home stressors: A daily diary study of mindfulness and anticipatory coping}, volume={187}, ISSN={0191-8869}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111399}, DOI={10.1016/j.paid.2021.111399}, abstractNote={Anticipatory coping involves efforts to reduce the consequences of upcoming, future stressors, but the individual and contextual predictors of anticipatory coping are not well understood. The current study focuses on individual differences in age and income and contextual fluctuations in mindfulness (present-centered thinking) to predict daily fluctuations in anticipatory coping with future home-related stressors. In the Mindfulness and Anticipatory Coping Everyday study, 223 adults (116 aged 60–90, 107 aged 18–36) completed an online questionnaire for nine consecutive days via Qualtrics, providing demographic information on Day 1. For the subsequent 8 days, participants reported mindfulness and anticipatory coping. Multilevel models revealed that within-person increases in daily mindfulness were related to decreases in daily anticipatory coping among younger adults and higher income older adults. This research provides evidence of tension between mindfulness and future-oriented coping and shows how this depends on one's life stage and access to resources.}, journal={Personality and Individual Differences}, publisher={Elsevier BV}, author={Cobler, Morgan D. and Smith, Emily L. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2022}, month={Mar}, pages={111399} } @article{early_smith_neupert_2022, title={Age, education, and political involvement differences in daily election-related stress}, volume={5}, ISSN={1046-1310 1936-4733}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-02979-2}, DOI={10.1007/s12144-022-02979-2}, abstractNote={Stress in daily life is rather common, but elections can present unique challenges. Evaluating the impact of individual characteristics, behaviors, and political beliefs on stress processes is imperative to understanding how elections influence psychological well-being. Exploring how these individual and behavioral characteristics interacted to predict exposure to election-related stressors, we hypothesized that age, education, and past socio-political involvement would be associated with exposure to election-related stressors. In the 2018 U.S. Midterm Election Stress Coping and Prevention Every Day (ESCAPED) study, 140 participants in the United States and territories aged 19–86 were recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk for a 30-day daily diary study. Collectively, participants completed a total of 1196 reports between October 15, 2018 and November 13, 2018. The midterm election was November 6, 2018. Each day, participants reported on past political participation, election stress anticipation, and exposure to election-related stressors. Confirming our hypothesis, on days when people were more politically active and on days when stress anticipation increased, exposure to election-related stressors increased. Age differences in exposure depended on political activity in the last 24 h, with older adults exhibiting a steeper increase in exposure following political activity, especially if they were highly educated. However, higher education was protective against election-related stressors among younger adults even with increases in political activity. Individuals’ experiences, characteristics, and daily decisions influence the likelihood of exposure to election-related stressors. Additionally, for younger adults, education may function as a protective factor when they engage in political activities.}, journal={Current Psychology}, publisher={Springer Science and Business Media LLC}, author={Early, Alexandra S. and Smith, Emily L. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2022}, month={May} } @article{hess_neupert_lothary_2022, title={Aging attitudes and changes in the costs of cognitive engagement in older adults over 5 years.}, volume={37}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000685}, DOI={10.1037/pag0000685}, abstractNote={The costs associated with performing a specific activity may play an important role in determining engagement, potentially impacting the willingness of older adults to engage in activities-particularly those with high demands-that may have benefits for physical and cognitive health. The present study examined changes in both objective (effort expenditure) and subjective (perceived task demands) costs associated with engagement in a cognitively challenging memory-scan task across a 5-year period in 163 older adults aged 64-81. Consistent with assumptions of selective engagement theory (SET; Hess, 2014), the effort associated with performing the task-as assessed by systolic blood pressure responses-was observed to increase over time. Subjective assessments of costs, as assessed by the National Aeronautics and Space Administration task load index (NASA TLX), were also observed to increase. In addition, the increases in both objective and subjective costs were greatest at high levels of task demands. We further found that both the effort and, to a lesser extent, perceived costs associated with performance were moderated by aging attitudes. Specifically, more positive attitudes were associated with greater effort expenditure and lower perceived costs. These findings suggest that negative attitudes about aging may have detrimental effects on perceived ability to perform challenging tasks. Given the importance of costs in determining activity participation, promoting positive aging attitudes along with more realistic perceptions of task demands may provide important means for promoting engagement in beneficial activities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).}, number={4}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Hess, Thomas M. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Lothary, Allura F.}, year={2022}, month={Jun}, pages={456–468} } @article{zhu_neupert_2022, title={Anticipatory stress during an election: A daily diary study}, volume={57}, ISSN={0020-7594 1464-066X}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12852}, DOI={10.1002/ijop.12852}, abstractNote={Previous research on election stress has focused on reacting to stressors that already occurred, whereas the forecasting of future stressors and associated responses have been underexamined. Leveraging the 2018 U.S. mid‐term election, we examined anticipatory stress response, operationalised as the within‐person association between daily stressors forecasting and negative affect (NA). We also explore whether such responses might be related to time, partisanship and political orientation. Participants were 125 adults in the U.S. who provided 1056 daily reports in a 29‐day daily diary study surrounding the election. Results indicated that daily forecasts of election stressors contributed to increased NA independent of election stressor exposure. Election stressor forecasting was more pronounced during pre‐election days and the election day than post‐election days, as well as greater in conservatives than liberals. Coping with anticipatory stress may be important for managing election stress.}, number={5}, journal={International Journal of Psychology}, publisher={Wiley}, author={Zhu, Xianghe and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2022}, month={May}, pages={629–633} } @inbook{neupert_bellingtier_2022, place={Cham, Switzerland}, series={International Perspectives on Aging}, title={Between- and Within-Person Approaches to Subjective Views of Aging}, ISBN={9783031110726 9783031110733}, ISSN={2197-5841 2197-585X}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-11073-3_11}, DOI={10.1007/978-3-031-11073-3_11}, abstractNote={Our primary objective is to synthesize the current state of the field regarding between- and within-person processes in subjective views of aging and to suggest areas for future directions. We combine data from three distinct daily diary datasets to describe within-person fluctuations in subjective aging from adolescence through old age. We underscore the need to concurrently examine between- and within-person processes, especially as they interact to reveal person and environment interactions. For example, individual differences in aging attitudes interact with daily experiences of age-related change to predict daily emotional and cognitive well-being. Those who generally feel more positive about their own aging are actually more vulnerable to daily increases in loss-based age-related experiences. We draw on Bronfenbrenner’s (Am Psychol 32(7): 513–531, 1977) ecological model and synthesize it with Diehl et al.’s (Dev Rev 34(2): 93–113, 2014) model of awareness of aging to highlight the importance of continuing to investigate person and environment interactions across multiple systems of influence. We challenge the field to think and act globally as the majority of our current findings come from W.E.I.R.D. (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, Democratic) cultures.}, booktitle={Subjective Views of Aging}, publisher={Springer International Publishing}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Bellingtier, Jennifer A.}, editor={Palgi, Y. and Shira, A. and Diehl, M.Editors}, year={2022}, pages={187–207}, collection={International Perspectives on Aging} } @article{neupert_2022, title={Mental Health, Stress and Student Well-Being}, url={https://doi.org/10.52750/300976}, DOI={10.52750/300976}, abstractNote={Everyone experiences stress.But the types of stress that people experience and the ways that people respond to stresses differ.Stress can result from major life events (like death of a parent) or from chronic, ongoing conditions (like the COVID-19 pandemic) and from daily hassles (like arguments).Stress is typically associated with worse mental and physical health, but the negative consequences of stress depend on the person and the situation.Dr. Neupert shares some of her research that highlights for whom and under which circumstances well-being can be optimized in times of stress.She discusses differences in risk and resiliency factors for COVID-19-related stress, significant stress-related challenges faced by healthcare professionals during COVID-19, how planning ahead for the future can prevent stress.She also discusses how trying to be mindful (staying focused on the present moment) helps to avoid the negative emotional consequences of stress.}, author={Neupert, Shevaun}, year={2022}, month={Jun} } @article{dunsmore_neupert_2022, title={No Pain, No Gain? Personality Associations With Awareness of Aging Depend on Arthritis}, volume={13}, ISSN={1664-1078}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.863152}, DOI={10.3389/fpsyg.2022.863152}, abstractNote={BackgroundAwareness of aging brings to light one’s own perceived behavioral, physical, and cognitive changes associated with getting older. Personality and physical illness are each related to two components of awareness of aging: attitudes toward own aging (ATOA), and awareness of age-related changes (AARC). Here, we move beyond main effects to examine how personality and arthritis interact with respect to awareness of aging.}, journal={Frontiers in Psychology}, publisher={Frontiers Media SA}, author={Dunsmore, Victoria J. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2022}, month={Jun} } @article{halberstadt_cooke_garner_hughes_oertwig_neupert_2022, title={Racialized emotion recognition accuracy and anger bias of children’s faces.}, volume={22}, ISSN={1931-1516 1528-3542}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000756}, DOI={10.1037/emo0000756}, abstractNote={Research suggests that individuals are racially biased when judging the emotions of others (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002) and particularly regarding attributions about the emotion of anger (Halberstadt, Castro, Chu, Lozada, & Sims, 2018; Hugenberg & Bodenhausen, 2003). Systematic, balanced designs are rare, and are comprised of adults viewing adults. The present study expands the questions of racialized emotion recognition accuracy and anger bias to the world of children. Findings that adults demonstrate either less emotion accuracy and/or greater anger bias for Black versus White children could potentially explain some of the large racialized disciplinary discrepancies in schools. To test whether racialized emotion recognition accuracy and anger bias toward children exists, we asked 178 prospective teachers to complete an emotion recognition task comprised of 72 children's facial expressions depicting six emotions and divided equally by race (Black, White) and gender (female, male). We also assessed implicit bias via the child race Implicit Association Test and explicit bias via questionnaire. Multilevel modeling revealed nuanced racialized emotion recognition accuracy with a race by gender interaction, but clear racialized anger bias toward both Black boys and girls. Both Black boys and Black girls were falsely seen as angry more often than White boys and White girls. Higher levels of either implicit or explicit bias did not increase odds of Black children being victim to anger bias, but instead decreased odds that White children would be misperceived as angry. Implications for addressing preexisting biases in teacher preparation programs and by children and parents are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).}, number={3}, journal={Emotion}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Halberstadt, Amy G. and Cooke, Alison N. and Garner, Pamela W. and Hughes, Sherick A. and Oertwig, Dejah and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2022}, month={Apr}, pages={403–417} } @article{archibald_neupert_2022, title={“Keeping Up with the Joneses”: Subjective Social Status Predicts Proactive Coping}, volume={29}, ISSN={1068-0667 1573-3440}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10804-022-09392-w}, DOI={10.1007/s10804-022-09392-w}, number={2}, journal={Journal of Adult Development}, publisher={Springer Science and Business Media LLC}, author={Archibald, Jessica L. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2022}, month={Jan}, pages={171–178} } @article{hughes_coblenz_smith_neupert_pearman_2021, title={AAA Research Methodology I: Overcoming Linguistic and Cultural Barriers in Aging Research Among Asians: Presenter Discussion (vol 4, pg 963, 2020)}, volume={5}, ISSN={["2399-5300"]}, DOI={10.1093/geroni/igab007}, abstractNote={Slowing the spread of COVID-19 depends on public adherence to precautionary actions, such as wearing masks The Health Belief Model (Rosenstock, 1974) suggests the likelihood of using precautionary measures depends on perceived susceptibility, the severity of a disease, and whether effective measures can be taken to reduce the perceived threat of a disease This daily diary study focused on identifying predictors of daily precautionary behavior in older persons Between April 1 and June 26, 2020, 261 adults ages 55-79 (M = 64 29, SD = 5 20) completed up to 30 consecutive days of online diaries We examined whether perceived risk, COVID-19 knowledge, fake news beliefs, information seeking, disruption to routine, in-person interactions, and leaving the house predicted the number of daily precautions participants engaged in Multilevel modeling was used to examine within-person fluctuations in precautions as well as change in precautions from one day to the next People who reported higher education, scored higher on the COVID-19 knowledge quiz, had lower fake news beliefs, and perceived a higher risk of contracting COVID-19 endorsed more precautions At the daily level, increases in the number of in-person interactions, leaving home, and disruption to daily routine were each associated with decreases in precautionary behaviors Concurrent day and lagged models showed significant interactions between information seeking and perceived risk, suggesting increases in information seeking are related to increases in precautions for those who consider their risk to be low Findings highlight potentially intervenable factors that influence older adults’ daily decision making related to precautionary actions}, number={2}, journal={INNOVATION IN AGING}, author={Hughes, MacKenzie and Coblenz, Clara and Smith, Emily and Neupert, Shevaun and Pearman, Ann}, year={2021} } @article{pearman_neupert_ennis_2021, title={Age and extraversion differences in heart rate reactivity during working memory tasks}, volume={16}, ISSN={1932-6203}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245539}, DOI={10.1371/journal.pone.0245539}, abstractNote={Research and theory have shown a link between heart rate reactivity during cognitive testing and extraversion in younger adults; however, similar work has not been conducted with older adults. This study was designed to explore age and extraversion-related differences in within-person heart rate (HR) reactivity during two working memory tasks of varying difficulty using a multi-level modeling approach. Across 570 total within-person assessments of continuous HR monitoring, 28 younger adults (M = 19.76, SD = 1.15) and 29 older adults (M = 71.19, SD = 6.63) were administered two working memory tasks (backward digit span and n-back). There were no age differences in reactivity during the backward digit span. However, similar to previous findings, on the more difficult n-back task, younger adults low in extraversion showed a trend toward higher HR reactivity than young adults high in extraversion. Interestingly, the older adults showed the opposite pattern in that lower extraversion older adults were less reactive than the higher extraversion older adults who showed the steepest increase in HR. The HR increase of the older adults high in extraversion may be an indication of higher engagement in this more difficult task. Individual differences in extraversion need to be taken into account when administering working memory tasks in older adults.}, number={1}, journal={PLOS ONE}, publisher={Public Library of Science (PLoS)}, author={Pearman, Ann and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Ennis, Gilda E.}, editor={Boylan, Jennifer MorozinkEditor}, year={2021}, month={Jan}, pages={e0245539} } @article{neupert_2021, title={Anticipatory Coping Diversity: Implications for Emotional, Physical, and Cognitive Reactivity to Daily Stressors}, volume={77}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab169}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbab169}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={4}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D}, editor={Hoppmann, ChristianeEditor}, year={2021}, month={Sep}, pages={721–732} } @article{neupert_growney_zhu_sorensen_smith_hannig_2021, title={BFF: Bayesian, Fiducial, and Frequentist Analysis of Cognitive Engagement among Cognitively Impaired Older Adults}, volume={23}, ISSN={1099-4300}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/e23040428}, DOI={10.3390/e23040428}, abstractNote={Engagement in cognitively demanding activities is beneficial to preserving cognitive health. Our goal was to demonstrate the utility of frequentist, Bayesian, and fiducial statistical methods for evaluating the robustness of effects in identifying factors that contribute to cognitive engagement for older adults experiencing cognitive decline. We collected a total of 504 observations across two longitudinal waves of data from 28 cognitively impaired older adults. Participants’ systolic blood pressure responsivity, an index of cognitive engagement, was continuously sampled during cognitive testing. Participants reported on physical and mental health challenges and provided hair samples to assess chronic stress at each wave. Using the three statistical paradigms, we compared results from six model testing levels and longitudinal changes in health and stress predicting changes in cognitive engagement. Findings were mostly consistent across the three paradigms, providing additional confidence in determining effects. We extend selective engagement theory to cognitive impairment, noting that health challenges and stress appear to be important moderators. Further, we emphasize the utility of the Bayesian and fiducial paradigms for use with relatively small sample sizes because they are not based on asymptotic distributions. In particular, the fiducial paradigm is a useful tool because it provides more information than p values without the need to specify prior distributions, which may unduly influence the results based on a small sample. We provide the R code used to develop and implement all models.}, number={4}, journal={Entropy}, publisher={MDPI AG}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Growney, Claire M. and Zhu, Xianghe and Sorensen, Julia K. and Smith, Emily L. and Hannig, Jan}, year={2021}, month={Apr}, pages={428} } @article{kim_kim_neupert_boerner_2021, title={Changes in married older adults’ self-perceptions of aging: The role of gender.}, volume={36}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000507}, DOI={10.1037/pag0000507}, abstractNote={To what extent self-perceptions of aging and their correlates in later life may be gendered remains relatively unexplored. In particular, little is known about how changes in the health and spousal relationship quality over time contribute to self-perceptions of aging among married men and women. To clarify these links, we analyzed panel data from the Health and Retirement Study (2008-2016) on married individuals aged 65 years and older (N = 2,623) using within-between random effects models. Findings showed no gender difference in self-perceptions of aging at baseline and in the rate of change, and poorer health and spousal relationship quality were generally associated with less positive self-perceptions of aging. However, men and women differed in how within-person changes in health and spousal relationship quality were associated with their self-perceptions of aging. Increases in spousal strain and chronic conditions were associated with less positive self-perceptions of aging on that wave for men, whereas increases in functional limitations were associated with less positive self-perceptions of aging on that wave for women. Finally, a person-mean of spousal strain had a moderating effect for men, such that men with more overall spousal strain reported less positive self-perceptions of aging across a range of chronic conditions, compared to the men with less overall spousal strain. Findings highlight the intersection of social resources, health, and self-perceptions of aging, suggesting that gender differences in older adults' self-perceptions of aging are contextualized by different behaviors and social experiences among married men and women. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).}, number={3}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Kim, Yijung K. and Kim, Kyungmin and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Boerner, Kathrin}, year={2021}, month={May}, pages={383–393} } @article{zhu_neupert_2021, title={Core Beliefs Disruption in the Context of an Election: Implications for Subjective Well-Being}, volume={125}, ISSN={0033-2941 1558-691X}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00332941211021347}, DOI={10.1177/00332941211021347}, abstractNote={ Major life events often challenge the core beliefs people hold about the world, which is a crucial cognitive process predictive of adjustment outcomes. Elections have been associated with physical and socioemotional responses, but what is unclear is whether core beliefs can be disrupted and what implication this disruption might have for well-being. In two studies, we examined the association between core beliefs disruption and well-being in the context of the 2018 U.S. midterm election. In both studies, participants reported a small degree of disruption of core beliefs due to the election. In Study 1, a 14-day daily diary study spanning the weeks before and after the election, multilevel modeling on 529 daily reports revealed that greater disruption of core beliefs was associated with lower mean levels of life satisfaction and greater changes in positive and negative affect. In Study 2, a cross-sectional study conducted 40 days following the election, linear regression analyses on 767 adults aged 18–77 from all 50 states revealed that the disruption of core beliefs due to the midterm election was positively associated with current life satisfaction. The effect held when controlling for multiple confounding factors. These findings suggest that elections can trigger disruption of core beliefs, and this disruption may spill over to subjective well-being in the short term but may positively contribute to post-election adjustment. }, number={5}, journal={Psychological Reports}, publisher={SAGE Publications}, author={Zhu, Xianghe and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2021}, month={Jun}, pages={2546–2570} } @article{bellingtier_rupprecht_neupert_lang_2021, title={Daily experiences of subjective age discordance and well-being.}, volume={36}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000621}, DOI={10.1037/pag0000621}, abstractNote={Subjective age discordance (SAD) captures the difference between how old one feels and how old one would ideally like to be. We investigated the presence, strength, and fluctuation of this discordance in daily life as well as its relationship to various indicators of physical and psychological well-being with an 8-day diary study. Participants were 116 older and 107 younger adults who completed daily measures of felt age, ideal age, positive and negative affect, physical symptoms, and stressors. We operationalized SAD as felt age minus ideal age divided by chronological age and compared the utility of this discordance to the more established proportional discrepancy of felt age from chronological age. Daily SAD was present in both age groups, such that individuals idealized younger ages than they felt. This discordance was larger in older than younger adults, although younger adults exhibited more daily fluctuations in SAD. Within-person increases in SAD were associated with lower positive affect, whereas larger SAD at the between-person level was associated with more physical symptoms and stressors. These relationships were over and above the associations of felt and chronological age with the outcomes suggesting the utility of daily SAD for understanding daily physical and psychological well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).}, number={6}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Bellingtier, Jennifer A. and Rupprecht, Fiona S. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Lang, Frieder R.}, year={2021}, month={Sep}, pages={744–751} } @article{diachina_neupert_2021, title={Daily stressor forecasting and anticipatory coping: Within-Person processes in age differences in positive emotional reactivity}, volume={26}, ISSN={1360-7863 1364-6915}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2021.1998353}, DOI={10.1080/13607863.2021.1998353}, abstractNote={Abstract Objectives: Whereas previous work has almost exclusively focused on negative emotional reactivity to daily stressors, we focus on positive emotional reactivity - changes in positive emotions in response to a stressor. We examine daily forecasting and anticipatory coping for next-day home-related stressors within the temporal space before stressors to examine positive emotional reactivity in younger and older adults. Methods: We recruited 107 younger (M age = 19.44, range 18-36) and 116 older (M age = 64.71, range 60-90) participants who completed a nine day daily online diary for 1,627 total days. Participants reported demographics on Day 1 and then stressor forecasts, anticipatory coping, stressor exposure, and positive affect on Days 2-9. Results: Older adults had more positive affect when using outcome fantasy for next-day home stressors compared to younger adults, but forecasting a home stressor was associated with a stronger increase in positive affect for younger adults compared to older adults. Conclusion: These results reveal complex and dynamic links that exist in age differences in stressor forecasting, anticipatory coping, and their association with positive affect. The context of daily stressors is important to understand the nuances of matching stressor forecasts and anticipatory coping for positive emotional well-being in younger and older adults.}, number={12}, journal={Aging & Mental Health}, publisher={Informa UK Limited}, author={Diachina, Allison K. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2021}, month={Nov}, pages={2407–2415} } @article{smith_anglis_neupert_2021, title={Delinquency differences in daily emotional reactivity to mindfulness lapses}, volume={6}, ISSN={1046-1310 1936-4733}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01945-8}, DOI={10.1007/s12144-021-01945-8}, journal={Current Psychology}, publisher={Springer Science and Business Media LLC}, author={Smith, Emily L. and Anglis, Elizabeth M. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2021}, month={Jun} } @article{neupert_bellingtier_smith_2021, title={Emotional reactivity changes to daily stressors surrounding the 2016 U.S. presidential election}, volume={40}, ISSN={1046-1310 1936-4733}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00215-y}, DOI={10.1007/s12144-019-00215-y}, number={6}, journal={Current Psychology}, publisher={Springer Science and Business Media LLC}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Bellingtier, Jennifer A. and Smith, Emily L.}, year={2021}, month={Mar}, pages={2832–2842} } @article{pearman_hughes_coblenz_smith_neupert_2021, title={Experiencing and Forecasting COVID-19 Daily Stress on Mental Health Reactivity Across Age and Race}, volume={77}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab197}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbab197}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={4}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Pearman, Ann and Hughes, MacKenzie L and Coblenz, Clara W and Smith, Emily L and Neupert, Shevaun D}, editor={Luong, GloriaEditor}, year={2021}, month={Dec}, pages={e16–e22} } @article{dubljević_neupert_2021, title={The complex nature of willpower and conceptual mapping of its normative significance in research on stress, addiction, and dementia}, volume={44}, ISSN={0140-525X 1469-1825}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X20000886}, DOI={10.1017/S0140525X20000886}, abstractNote={Abstract}, journal={Behavioral and Brain Sciences}, publisher={Cambridge University Press (CUP)}, author={Dubljević, Veljko and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2021} } @article{palgi_shrira_neupert_2021, title={Views on Aging and Health: A Multidimensional and Multitemporal Perspective}, volume={76}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbab026}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbab026}, number={5}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Palgi, Yuval and Shrira, Amit and Neupert, Shevaun D}, year={2021}, month={Mar}, pages={821–824} } @article{pearman_hughes_smith_neupert_2020, title={Age Differences in Risk and Resilience Factors in COVID-19-Related Stress}, volume={76}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa120}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbaa120}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={2}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Pearman, Ann and Hughes, MacKenzie L and Smith, Emily L and Neupert, Shevaun D}, editor={Fiske, AmyEditor}, year={2020}, month={Aug}, pages={e38–e44} } @article{ayalon_chasteen_diehl_levy_neupert_rothermund_tesch-römer_wahl_2020, title={Aging in Times of the COVID-19 Pandemic: Avoiding Ageism and Fostering Intergenerational Solidarity}, volume={76}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa051}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbaa051}, abstractNote={1School of Social Work, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel. 2Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada. 3Department of Human Development and Family Studies, Colorado State University, Fort Collins. 4Social and Behavioral Sciences Department, Yale School of Public Health, New Haven, Connecticut. 5Department of Psychology, North Carolina State University, Raleigh. 6Department of Psychology, Friedrich-Schiller-University Jena, Germany. 7German Centre of Gerontology, Berlin, Germany. 8Network of Aging Research, Heidelberg University, Germany.}, number={2}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Ayalon, Liat and Chasteen, Alison and Diehl, Manfred and Levy, Becca R and Neupert, Shevaun D and Rothermund, Klaus and Tesch-Römer, Clemens and Wahl, Hans-Werner}, year={2020}, month={Apr}, pages={e49–e52} } @article{o’brien_torres_neupert_2020, title={Cognitive Interference in the Context of Daily Stressors, Daily Awareness of Age-Related Change, and General Aging Attitudes}, volume={76}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa155}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbaa155}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={5}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={O’Brien, Erica L and Torres, Genesis E and Neupert, Shevaun D}, editor={Palgi, YuvalEditor}, year={2020}, month={Sep}, pages={920–929} } @misc{neupert_bellingtier_2020, title={Control and Cognition: Contextual and Individual Differences in Cognitive Aging}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/9781108552684.032}, DOI={10.1017/9781108552684.032}, abstractNote={Sense of control over one’s life declines in the later portion of the life span, which is not surprising in the face of increased losses and decreased gains associated with aging. Unfortunately, the maintenance of sense of control is a key indicator of successful aging while low control beliefs are a risk factor for poor aging-related outcomes, such as lower concurrent and subsequent cognitive functioning. The simultaneous focus on the person and the environment is an important characteristic of research on control beliefs. We synthesize the state of the field and discuss the current understanding of the complex interplay of control beliefs and cognition. In addition, we propose that awareness of aging, which is the subjective interpretation of aging, may be an important future direction to elucidate the control-cognition relationships.}, journal={The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Aging}, publisher={Cambridge University Press}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Bellingtier, Jennifer A.}, year={2020}, month={May}, pages={514–531} } @article{smith_neupert_2020, title={Daily Stressor Exposure: Examining Interactions of Delinquent Networks, Daily Mindfulness and Control among Emerging Adults}, volume={41}, ISSN={1046-1310 1936-4733}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00694-4}, DOI={10.1007/s12144-020-00694-4}, number={4}, journal={Current Psychology}, publisher={Springer Science and Business Media LLC}, author={Smith, Emily L. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2020}, month={Mar}, pages={1912–1920} } @article{zhu_neupert_2020, title={Dynamic awareness of age‐related losses predict concurrent and subsequent changes in daily inductive reasoning performance}, volume={39}, ISSN={0261-510X 2044-835X}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12344}, DOI={10.1111/bjdp.12344}, abstractNote={To address gaps in previous research, we examined daily within‐person associations between awareness of age‐related change (AARC) and cognitive performance in older adults. One hundred twelve adults aged 60–90 participated in an online daily diary study for nine consecutive days. On Day 1, they reported demographic information. On Days 2–9, they reported daily AARC, subjective age, and stressors and completed three tasks that gauged memory (word recall), perceptual speed (number comparison), and reasoning (letter series), respectively, once per day. Unconditional multilevel models revealed significant within‐person fluctuations in daily cognitive performance and daily AARC. Controlling for age, education, health, and daily stressors, daily awareness of age‐related losses was negatively associated with letter series scores of the same day and changes from one day to the next. The effects held over and above AARC gains and subjective age. Dynamic, naturally occurring perceptions of ageing may influence within‐person concurrent and subsequent changes in reasoning performance on a daily basis and point to a promising avenue of intervention.}, number={2}, journal={British Journal of Developmental Psychology}, publisher={Wiley}, author={Zhu, Xianghe and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2020}, month={Jul}, pages={282–298} } @article{bellingtier_neupert_2020, title={Feeling Young and in Control: Daily Control Beliefs Are Associated With Younger Subjective Ages}, volume={75}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz015}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbz015}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={5}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Bellingtier, Jennifer A and Neupert, Shevaun D}, editor={Diehl, ManfredEditor}, year={2020}, month={Jun}, pages={e13–e17} } @article{neupert_zhu_2020, title={Heterogeneity In Aging: The Need For A Tripartite Contextual Approach}, volume={61}, ISSN={0016-9013 1758-5341}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnaa127}, DOI={10.1093/geront/gnaa127}, number={1}, journal={The Gerontologist}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D and Zhu, Xianghe}, year={2020}, month={Oct}, pages={132–133} } @article{pearman_hughes_smith_neupert_2020, title={Mental Health Challenges of United States Healthcare Professionals During COVID-19}, volume={11}, ISBN={1664-1078}, ISSN={1664-1078}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02065}, DOI={10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02065}, abstractNote={As COVID-19 continues to impact global society, healthcare professionals (HCPs) are at risk for a number of negative well-being outcomes due to their role as care providers. The objective of this study was to better understand the current psychological impact of COVID-19 on HCPs in the United States This study used an online survey tool to collect demographic data and measures of well-being of adults age 18 and older living in the United States between March 20, 2020 and May 14, 2020. Measures included anxiety and stress related to COVID-19, depressive symptoms, current general anxiety, health questions, tiredness, control beliefs, proactive coping, and past and future appraisals of COVID-related stress. The sample included 90 HCPs and 90 age-matched controls (Mage = 34.72 years, SD = 9.84, range = 23 – 67) from 35 states of the United States. A multivariate analysis of variance was performed, using education as a covariate, to identify group differences in the mental and physical health measures. HCPs reported higher levels of depressive symptoms, past and future appraisal of COVID-related stress, concern about their health, tiredness, current general anxiety, and constraint, in addition to lower levels of proactive coping compared to those who were not HCPs (p < 0.001, η2 = 0.28). Within the context of this pandemic, HCPs were at increased risk for a number of negative well-being outcomes. Potential targets, such as adaptive coping training, for intervention are discussed.}, journal={Frontiers in Psychology}, publisher={Frontiers Media SA}, author={Pearman, Ann and Hughes, MacKenzie L. and Smith, Emily L. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2020}, month={Aug} } @article{yorgason_choi_neupert_cichy_hill_2020, title={Microlongitudinal analysis of memory failures, negative affect, and marital interactions.}, volume={35}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000400}, DOI={10.1037/pag0000400}, abstractNote={Many older adults experience age-associated memory changes. Scholars have explored more intensive memory loss problems (i.e., dementia) within marriage, however minor memory failures may also impact older adults' relationships. Minor daily memory failures could bring frustrations that manifest as negative affect, which could then spill over into marital interactions. The current study utilized a daily framework to examine microlongitudinal pathways of memory to negative affect to marital interactions among 191 couples across 14 consecutive days. Dyadic multivariate multilevel structural equation models were used to explore daily positive and negative marital interactions as a function of 2 days prior (t-2) memory failures and prior day (t-1) negative affect. Findings suggest that between-person memory failures were consistently linked with negative affect, and between-person negative affect was linked to fewer daily positive and more daily negative marital interactions for husbands, as well as more daily negative marital interactions for wives. Within-person t-2 memory failures were linked to t-1 negative affect for husbands. Indirect associations linking memory failures with negative marital quality through negative affect were significant for wives, and only at a between-person level. The effects of daily memory failures have important implications for daily negative affect and negative marital interactions, and these effects may last for multiple days. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).}, number={1}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Yorgason, Jeremy B. and Choi, Heejeong and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Cichy, Kelly E. and Hill, Melanie S.}, year={2020}, month={Feb}, pages={8–19} } @article{neupert_2020, title={Pandemic Stressbusters}, url={https://web.ncsu.edu/accolades-magazine/2020/10/15/quiz-pandemic-stress-busters/?utm_campaign=accolades-fall-2020&utm_medium=email&utm_source=newsletter&utm_content=chass-email-first-distribution&utm_term=quiz}, journal={NC State Accolades}, publisher={NC State Accolades}, author={Neupert, S.D.}, year={2020}, month={Oct} } @article{pearman_neupert_hughes_2020, title={State Anxiety Is Related to Cortisol Response During Cognitive Testing for Older Adults}, volume={6}, ISSN={2333-7214 2333-7214}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2333721420914776}, DOI={10.1177/2333721420914776}, abstractNote={ Cognitive testing situations can be stressful for both younger and older adults, but threats of cognitive evaluation may be particularly salient among anxious older individuals as they tend to be more concerned than younger adults about their cognitive abilities and age-related cognitive decline. We examined age-related differences in the effect of anxiety on cortisol responses during cognitive testing in a sample of 27 younger ( M = 19.8) and 29 older ( M = 71.2) adults. Older adults with higher anxiety also had higher during-task cortisol (suggesting higher reactivity to testing) than older adults with lower anxiety and young adults. There was no effect of anxiety on cortisol for younger adults. Simultaneously examining subjective (state anxiety) and physiological (cortisol response) indicators of threat during cognitive testing appears to be especially important for older adults with higher state anxiety. The results are important for understanding cortisol reactivity, particularly in older adults. Researchers who administer cognitive tests to older adults and clinicians who work with older adults with cognitive concerns and/or anxiety may want to consider how they present their material. }, journal={Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine}, publisher={SAGE Publications}, author={Pearman, Ann and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Hughes, MacKenzie L.}, year={2020}, month={Jan}, pages={233372142091477} } @article{neupert_shipman_2020, title={There’s no “one size fits all” way to cope with stress right now}, url={https://www.futurity.org/covid-19-stress-coping-2322382/}, journal={Futurity}, author={Neupert, S.D. and Shipman, Matt}, year={2020}, month={Apr} } @article{polk_smith_zhang_neupert_2020, title={Thinking ahead and staying in the present: Implications for reactivity to daily stressors}, volume={161}, ISSN={0191-8869}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.109971}, DOI={10.1016/j.paid.2020.109971}, abstractNote={We examined how proactive coping and daily mindfulness may work together to predict emotional reactivity to daily stressors. Using data from the Mindfulness and Anticipatory Coping Everyday study (MACE; Neupert & Bellingtier, 2017; Neupert & Bellingtier, 2019), 116 older adults and 107 younger adults participated in a daily diary study for nine consecutive days. Results from multilevel models suggest that people high in proactive coping were more emotionally reactive to daily stressors on days with decreased mindfulness. Due to the trait-like future-oriented thinking of proactive coping and the state-like present-oriented aspect of daily mindfulness, these results underscore the importance of simultaneously considering state and trait information to elucidate antecedents, correlates, and consequences of daily stressors.}, journal={Personality and Individual Differences}, publisher={Elsevier BV}, author={Polk, Melody G. and Smith, Emily L. and Zhang, Ling-Rui and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2020}, month={Jul}, pages={109971} } @article{zhang_neupert_2020, title={Within- and Between-Person Relationships Among Health, Awareness of Aging, and Control Beliefs: A Microlongitudinal Study}, volume={76}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa180}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbaa180}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={5}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Zhang, Shenghao and Neupert, Shevaun D}, editor={Shrira, AmitEditor}, year={2020}, month={Oct}, pages={858–870} } @article{neupert_hannig_2019, title={BFF: Bayesian, Fiducial, Frequentist Analysis of Age Effects in Daily Diary Data}, volume={75}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz100}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbz100}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={1}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D and Hannig, Jan}, editor={Ram, NilamEditor}, year={2019}, month={Aug}, pages={67–79} } @article{hartsell_neupert_2019, title={Chronic and Daily Stressors Along With Negative Affect Interact to Predict Daily Tiredness}, volume={38}, ISSN={["1552-4523"]}, DOI={10.1177/0733464817741684}, abstractNote={ The present study examines the within-person relationship of daily stressors and tiredness and whether this depends on daily negative affect and individual differences in chronic stress. One hundred sixteen older adult participants were recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk for a 9-day daily diary study. Daily tiredness, daily stressors, and negative affect were measured each day, and chronic stress was measured at baseline. Daily stressors, daily negative affect, and chronic stress interacted to predict daily tiredness. People with high chronic stress who experienced an increase in daily negative affect were the most reactive to daily stressors in terms of experiencing an increase in daily tiredness. We also found that people with low levels of chronic stress were the most reactive to daily stressors when they experienced low levels of daily negative affect. Our results highlight the need for individualized and contextualized approaches to combating daily tiredness in older adults. }, number={12}, journal={JOURNAL OF APPLIED GERONTOLOGY}, author={Hartsell, Elizabeth N. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2019}, month={Dec}, pages={1728–1745} } @inbook{neupert_bellingtier_2019, place={New York, NY}, title={Daily Diary Designs in Lifespan Developmental Psychology}, booktitle={The Oxford encyclopedia of psychology and aging}, publisher={Oxford University Press}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Bellingtier, Jennifer A.}, editor={Knight, Bob G.Editor}, year={2019} } @article{zhang_gamaldo_neupert_allaire_2019, title={Predicting Control Beliefs in Older Adults: A Micro-Longitudinal Study}, volume={75}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz001}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbz001}, abstractNote={Abstract}, number={5}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Zhang, Shenghao and Gamaldo, Alyssa A and Neupert, Shevaun D and Allaire, Jason C}, editor={Infurna, Frank JEditor}, year={2019}, month={Jan}, pages={e1–e12} } @article{zottola_desmarais_neupert_dong_laber_lowder_van dorn_2019, title={Results of the Brief Jail Mental Health Screen Across Repeated Jail Bookings}, volume={70}, ISSN={1075-2730 1557-9700}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.201800377}, DOI={10.1176/appi.ps.201800377}, abstractNote={OBJECTIVE The Brief Jail Mental Health Screen (BJMHS) is widely used at intake in county jails to identify detainees who may have serious mental illness and who should be referred for further mental health evaluation. The BJMHS may be administered multiple times across repeated jail bookings; however, the extent to which results may change over time is unclear. To that end, the authors examined the odds of screening positive on the BJMHS across repeated jail bookings. METHODS Data were drawn from the administrative and medical records of a large, urban county jail that used the BJMHS at jail booking. The study sample comprised BJMHS results for the 12,531 jail detainees who were booked at least twice during the 3.5-year period (N=41,965 bookings). Multilevel logistic modeling was used to examine changes over time overall and within the four decision rules (current psychiatric medication, prior hospitalization, two or more current symptoms, and referral for any other reason). RESULTS Results show that the odds of a positive screen overall increased with each jail booking, as did the odds of referral for any other reason. In contrast, the odds of screening positive for two or more current symptoms and prior hospitalization decreased. There was no change in the odds of screening positive for current psychiatric medication across bookings. CONCLUSIONS Findings show that BJMHS results changed across bookings. Further research is needed to determine whether changes reflect true changes in mental health status, issues with fidelity, the repeated nature of the screening process, or other factors.}, number={11}, journal={Psychiatric Services}, publisher={American Psychiatric Association Publishing}, author={Zottola, Samantha A. and Desmarais, Sarah L. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Dong, Lin and Laber, Eric and Lowder, Evan M. and Van Dorn, Richard A.}, year={2019}, month={Nov}, pages={1006–1012} } @misc{neupert_2018, title={"It Depends”: the Psychology of Health, Stress and Daily Life}, url={https://news.ncsu.edu/2018/03/psych-daily-life-2018/}, journal={NCSU Research Matters}, author={Neupert, S.D.}, year={2018} } @article{neupert_neubauer_scott_hyun_sliwinski_2018, title={Back to the Future: Examining Age Differences in Processes Before Stressor Exposure}, volume={74}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby074}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gby074}, abstractNote={1Department of Psychology, North Carolina State University, Raleigh. 2Education and Human Development, German Institute for International Educational Research (DIPF), Frankfurt am Main, Germany. 3Center for Research on Individual Development and Adaptive Education of Children at Risk (IDeA), Frankfurt am Main, Germany. 4Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, New York. 5Department of Human Development and Family Studies and Center for Healthy Aging, Pennsylvania State University, University Park.}, number={1}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D and Neubauer, Andreas B and Scott, Stacey B and Hyun, Jinshil and Sliwinski, Martin J}, year={2018}, month={Jul}, pages={1–6} } @misc{neupert_bellingtier_2018, title={Daily Diary Designs in Lifespan Developmental Psychology}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.347}, DOI={10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.347}, abstractNote={Daily diary designs allow researchers to examine processes that change together on a daily basis, often in a naturalistic setting. By studying within-person covariation between daily processes, one can more precisely establish the short-term effects and temporal ordering of concrete daily experiences. Additionally, the daily diary design reduces retrospective recall bias because participants are asked to recall events that occurred over the previous 24-hour period as opposed to a week or even a year. Therefore, a more accurate picture of individuals’ daily lives can be captured with this design. When conclusions are drawn between people about the relationship between the predictors and outcomes, the covariation that occurs within people through time is lost. In a within-person design, conclusions can be made about the simultaneous effects of within-person covariation as well as between-person differences. This is especially important when many interindividual differences (e.g., traits) may exist in within-person relationships (e.g., states).}, journal={Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology}, publisher={Oxford University Press}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Bellingtier, Jennifer A.}, year={2018}, month={Mar} } @article{neupert_bellingtier_2018, title={Daily Stressor Forecasts and Anticipatory Coping: Age Differences in Dynamic, Domain-Specific Processes}, volume={74}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby043}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gby043}, abstractNote={Objective We focused on the temporal space before stressor exposure and examined two constructs-daily stressor forecasting and anticipatory coping-for daily emotional well-being. Method One hundred and seven younger (M age = 19.44, range 18-36) and 116 older (M age = 64.71, range 60-90) participants reported on 1,627 total days via an online daily diary study. Participants reported baseline demographic information (Day 1) and stressor forecasts, anticipatory coping, stressor exposure, and negative affect (Days 2-9). Results We found significant intraindividual variability in stressor forecasts. Increases in forecasts of upcoming stressors were associated with increases in anticipatory coping of those stressors in some domains. Older adults forecasted more upcoming home stressors than younger adults, but older adults reported less anticipatory coping than younger adults. Finally, we found age differences in emotional reactivity to daily home stressors depending on previous-day forecasts and coping of those home stressors. Forecasting home stressors was associated with a stronger reduction in reactivity for younger adults relative to older adults, but stagnant deliberation coping was associated with increased reactivity for younger adults, not for older adults. Discussion Daily stressor forecasts are dynamic and situation-specific and linked to daily anticipatory coping and age differences in reactivity to home stressors.}, number={1}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D and Bellingtier, Jennifer A}, year={2018}, month={Apr}, pages={17–28} } @article{bellingtier_neupert_2018, title={Daily Subjective Age in Emerging Adults: “Now We’re Stressed Out”}, volume={7}, ISSN={2167-6968 2167-6984}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167696818785081}, DOI={10.1177/2167696818785081}, abstractNote={ During the emerging adulthood years, individuals explore their identities and often report feeling in-between adolescence and adulthood. These characteristics may correspond to greater variability in how old individuals feel. This study examined the daily variability in emerging adults’ subjective ages, as well as its association with daily stressors and the psychological dimensions of identity exploration and feeling in-between. Using a 9-day daily diary design, the study measured 106 emerging adults’ (18–22 years old) daily stressors, daily subjective ages, and endorsement of the Inventory of the Dimensions of Emerging Adulthood. Findings indicated that over half of the variability in emerging adults’ subjective ages occurred within person, and daily stressors could predict this variability. Furthermore, those high in identity exploration and feeling in-between were most likely to respond to daily stressors by feeling older. Results suggest that psychological identification with emerging adulthood amplifies responses to daily stressors and predicts feeling subjectively older. }, number={6}, journal={Emerging Adulthood}, publisher={SAGE Publications}, author={Bellingtier, Jennifer A. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2018}, month={Jun}, pages={468–477} } @article{english_bellingtier_neupert_2018, title={It’s “the Joneses”: the influence of objective and subjective socioeconomic status on subjective perceptions of aging}, volume={16}, ISSN={1613-9372 1613-9380}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-018-0475-2}, DOI={10.1007/s10433-018-0475-2}, abstractNote={Positive subjective perceptions of aging predict various well-being, physical health, mental health, and longevity outcomes. Thus, it is important to consider what factors contribute to their formation. Socioeconomic status (SES) has been hypothesized to be one such factor, but past research has been mixed. We propose that subjective assessments of SES may better predict subjective perceptions of aging than traditional objective measures (e.g., income and education). The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of objective and subjective indicators of SES on subjective perceptions of aging. Participants (n = 296) from the Mindfulness and Anticipatory Coping Everyday study were recruited with a Human Intelligence Task on Amazon's Mechanical Turk. Participants reported on their SES (i.e., income, education, and subjective social status) and subjective perceptions of aging (i.e., attitudes toward own aging, subjective age, and awareness of age-related gains and losses). Data were analyzed via hierarchical multiple regression with demographic and health variables entered first, followed by income and education, then subjective social status. Results demonstrated that although objective measures of SES did not contribute to predicting subjective perceptions of aging, those who rated themselves subjectively higher in their community social standing were more likely to possess positive aging attitudes, younger subjective ages, more awareness of age-related gains, and fewer awareness of age-related losses. These findings suggest that perceptions of doing better than one's neighbors (i.e., "the Joneses") may matter more for understanding subjective perceptions of aging than objective indicators of social status.}, number={1}, journal={European Journal of Ageing}, publisher={Springer Science and Business Media LLC}, author={English, Alaina N. and Bellingtier, Jennifer A. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2018}, month={May}, pages={121–128} } @article{bellingtier_neupert_2018, title={Negative Aging Attitudes Predict Greater Reactivity to Daily Stressors in Older Adults}, volume={73}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw086}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbw086}, abstractNote={Objectives In order to understand conflicting findings regarding the emotional reactions of older adults to daily stressors, we examined the possibility that negative aging attitudes could function as an important individual differences factor related to stressor reactivity. Method Using a daily dairy design, we examined the aging attitudes of 43 older adults reporting on 380 total days. Participants reported their aging attitudes on Day 1, followed by their stressor exposure and negative affect on Days 2-9. Covariates included age, gender, education, and personality. Results Using multilevel modeling, our results suggest that individuals with more positive aging attitudes report consistent levels of affect across study days regardless of stressors, whereas those with more negative aging attitudes reported increased emotional reactivity to daily stressors. Discussion Positive aging attitudes may serve as a resource that helps buffer reactions to daily stressors.}, number={7}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Bellingtier, Jennifer A. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2018}, month={Oct}, pages={1155–1159} } @misc{neupert_2018, title={Pack Hacks for Faculty: Hacking Work-Life Integration}, url={https://provost.ncsu.edu/news/2018/01/pack-hacks-for-faculty-hacking-work-life-integration/}, journal={Executive Vice Chancellor and Provost News}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2018} } @misc{neupert_bellingtier_2018, title={The Ups and Downs of Daily Diary Research}, ISBN={9781526446084}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781526446084}, DOI={10.4135/9781526446084}, publisher={SAGE Publications Ltd}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Bellingtier, Jennifer A.}, year={2018} } @article{hess_growney_o'brien_neupert_sherwood_2018, title={The role of cognitive costs, attitudes about aging, and intrinsic motivation in predicting engagement in everyday activities.}, volume={33}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000289}, DOI={10.1037/pag0000289}, abstractNote={Engagement in cognitively demanding everyday activities has been shown to benefit cognitive health in later life. We investigated the factors that influence engagement, with specific interest in determining the extent to which the costs of engaging cognitive resources are associated with intrinsic motivation and, ultimately, participation in everyday activities. Older adults (N = 153) aged from 65 to 81 years completed a challenging cognitive task, with the costs of cognitive engagement—operationalized as the effort required to maintain performance—assessed using systolic blood pressure responses (SBP-R). We also assessed participation in everyday activities using both 2-year retrospective reports and five daily reports over a 5-week period. Structural models revealed that lower levels of costs were associated with more positive attitudes about aging, which in turn were associated with higher levels of intrinsic motivation. Motivation was subsequently predictive of everyday activity engagement, with the effect being specific to those activities thought to place demands on cognitive resources. The measure of engagement had minimal impact on the nature of the observed effects, suggesting that the retrospective and weekly assessments were tapping into similar constructs. Taken together, the results are consistent with expectations derived from Selective Engagement Theory (Hess, 2014), which argues that engagement in demanding activities is related to the cost associated with such engagement, which in turn leads to selective participation through changes in motivation.}, number={6}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Hess, Thomas M. and Growney, Claire M. and O'Brien, Erica L. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Sherwood, Andrew}, year={2018}, month={Sep}, pages={953–964} } @article{neupert_bellingtier_2017, title={Aging Attitudes and Daily Awareness of Age-Related Change Interact to Predict Negative Affect}, volume={57}, ISSN={0016-9013 1758-5341}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnx055}, DOI={10.1093/geront/gnx055}, abstractNote={Background and Objectives Possessing more positive views of one's own aging is associated with better self-rated health, reduced reactivity to stressors, and better well-being. We examined two components of aging attitudes: awareness of age-related change (AARC) of loss and gain experiences and attitudes toward own aging (ATOA). We expected that AARC would vary day-to-day and interact with ATOA to predict daily negative affect. Research Design and Methods One hundred and sixteen participants (61% women, M age = 64.71, range 60-90) reported on 743 total days via an online daily diary study. On Day 1, participants reported baseline ATOA and baseline AARC for losses and gains. On Days 2-9, daily stressor exposure, daily AARC losses and gains, and negative affect were reported. Results Unconditional multilevel models revealed significant within-person fluctuation in daily AARC losses and gains. Controlling for daily stressors, age, and baseline AARC, daily increases in AARC losses were associated with increases in negative affect and a cross-level interaction revealed that this effect was stronger for those with more positive ATOA. Discussion and Implications AARC gains and losses vary from day-to-day, suggesting that interventions targeting the contextual fluctuations in daily life may be a promising avenue for future research. Specifically, individuals who feel generally positive about their own aging, although less likely to report awareness of daily age-related losses, may be the most vulnerable when they do occur. Efforts to reduce daily AARC losses (e.g., limiting activities due to age, receiving help because others assume age-related deficits) may improve the daily well-being of older adults.}, number={suppl_2}, journal={The Gerontologist}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Bellingtier, Jennifer A.}, year={2017}, month={Jun}, pages={S187–S192} } @article{morgan_desmarais_neupert_2017, title={An Integrated Model of Health and Happiness Among Post-9/11 Military Veterans}, volume={5}, ISSN={2163-5781 2163-5803}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21635781.2017.1310681}, DOI={10.1080/21635781.2017.1310681}, abstractNote={ABSTRACT The authors developed an integrated model of well-being, defined by happiness and physical health, among post-9/11 veterans, by examining associations of religious attendance, trauma, appreciation in life, and ease of readjustment to civilian life with well-being. Data on 712 post-9/11 military veterans (81.0% male) were drawn from the Pew Research Center's 2011 Veterans Study. The authors conducted multiple regression analyses to identify predictors of health and happiness and used structural equation modeling to develop an integrated model of veteran well-being. The authors' findings indicate that ease of readjustment to civilian life is a critical contributor to the health and happiness of veterans.}, number={3}, journal={Military Behavioral Health}, publisher={Informa UK Limited}, author={Morgan, Jessica Kelley and Desmarais, Sarah L. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2017}, month={Mar}, pages={236–244} } @article{neupert_desmarais_gray_cohn_doherty_knight_2017, title={Daily stressors as antecedents, correlates, and consequences of alcohol and drug use and cravings in community-based offenders.}, volume={31}, ISSN={1939-1501 0893-164X}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/adb0000276}, DOI={10.1037/adb0000276}, abstractNote={Justice-involved individuals with alcohol and drug use problems reoffend at higher rates than their nonusing counterparts, with alcohol and drug use serving as an important vector to recidivism. At the daily level, exposure to stressors may exacerbate problematic alcohol and drug use; at the individual level, prior treatment experiences may mitigate substance use as individuals adapt to and learn new coping mechanisms. We conducted a daily diary study using Interactive Voice Response technology over 14 consecutive days with 117 men on probation or parole participating in a community-based treatment program (n = 860 calls) and referred to medication-assisted treatment. Participants reported daily stressors, craving for alcohol and illegal drugs, and use of alcohol and illegal drugs 1 time each day. Results of multilevel models showed significant day-to-day fluctuation in alcohol and drug craving and use. In concurrent models, increases in daily stressors were associated with increases in cravings and use of illegal drugs. Prior treatment experience modified many of these relationships, and additional lagged models revealed that those with less treatment experience reported an increase in next-day alcohol craving when they experienced increases in stressors on the previous day compared to those with more treatment experience. Collectively, these findings highlight the importance of tailoring treatment as a function of individual differences, including prior treatment experiences, and targeting daily stressors and subsequent cravings among justice-involved adults with alcohol and drug use problems.}, number={3}, journal={Psychology of Addictive Behaviors}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Desmarais, Sarah L. and Gray, Julie S. and Cohn, Amy M. and Doherty, Stephen and Knight, Kevin}, year={2017}, month={May}, pages={315–325} } @article{neupert_carr_2017, title={Methodological Innovations in Gerontology: Advances in Psychosocial Research}, volume={73}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbx132}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbx132}, abstractNote={Forty years ago, Baltes, Reese, and Nesselroade (1977) published their path-breaking textbook Life-span Developmental Psychology: Introduction to Research Methods, which delineated methods for studying individual differences and changes over the life course. The textbook’s focus was explicitly interdisciplinary, emphasizing linkages among individual development, social contexts, and historical change. It is fitting that the articles featured in this special issue Methodological Innovations in Gerontology: Advances in Psychosocial Research bridge psychological and sociological principles. These papers exemplify the directive by Baltes and colleagues that “the complexity of the historical study of behavioral development within a changing biocultural context calls for unique methodologies and a heightened sensitivity to the pitfalls, blind alleys, and frustrations produced by malignant data” (1977: 13). Since the publication of the text, the data, methods, and technologies available to scholars of aging have expanded dramatically, enabling explorations of new areas of study, or more sophisticated investigations of questions at the core of social gerontology. Researchers have moved beyond data resources focused on one individual at a single or two points in time, and instead investigate the experiences of individuals embedded in dyads, families, social networks, and neighborhoods, at multiple points in time. Technological advances such as portable devices for experience sampling and daily diary studies have led to an increased volume of individual-, meta-, and macro-level data, necessitating the development and application of statistical techniques to appropriately model psychosocial phenomena. Merging traditional longitudinal designs, a hallmark of life course sociology, and within-person designs, the foundation of life-span psychology, has yielded measurement burst designs which allow for examinations of short-term fluctuations within the context of long-term changes. Detailed longitudinal data on large population-based samples enable scholars to explore the effects of complex life histories and trajectories on later-life outcomes. In this brief essay, we describe the key contributions of the 15 studies published in the issue, highlighting their innovations with respect to their unit of analysis, measurement, data collection practices, and statistical modeling, and their capacities for advancing methodological rigor and replicability.}, number={1}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D and Carr, Deborah}, year={2017}, month={Dec}, pages={1–4} } @article{burris_neupert_2017, title={Mood management: Mood repair and daily stressors correlate with daily affect}, volume={25}, journal={Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences}, author={Burris, E.N.u and Neupert, S.D.}, year={2017}, pages={27–32} } @article{lowder_desmarais_neupert_truelove_2017, title={SSI/SSDI Outreach, Access, and Recovery (SOAR): Disability Application Outcomes Among Homeless Adults}, volume={68}, ISSN={1075-2730 1557-9700}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.201600568}, DOI={10.1176/appi.ps.201600568}, abstractNote={OBJECTIVE The SSI/SSDI Outreach, Access, and Recovery (SOAR) program has been shown to increase access to Supplemental Security Income and Social Security Disability Insurance benefits among homeless adults. However, little empirical data exist on how or for whom SOAR achieves successful application outcomes. This study investigated applicant and application characteristics associated with disability application outcomes among homeless adults. METHODS Secondary data on 6,361 SOAR-assisted applications were obtained. Multilevel models investigated between-applicant differences in application processing time and decision as a function of applicant and application characteristics. RESULTS Older age and living in an institution were associated with greater odds of application approval. Female gender and receipt of public assistance were associated with longer processing time and lower odds of approval. Except for quality review, SOAR critical components were associated with greater odds of approval. CONCLUSIONS Women and adults receiving public assistance appear disadvantaged in the SOAR application process. SOAR critical components promote successful disability application outcomes.}, number={11}, journal={Psychiatric Services}, publisher={American Psychiatric Association Publishing}, author={Lowder, Evan M. and Desmarais, Sarah L. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Truelove, Melissa A.}, year={2017}, month={Nov}, pages={1189–1192} } @article{bellingtier_neupert_kotter-grühn_2017, title={The Combined Effects of Daily Stressors and Major Life Events on Daily Subjective Ages}, volume={72}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbv101}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbv101}, abstractNote={Objectives Stressors may be a contributing factor in determining how old an individual feels, looks, or would like to be. Currently, little research has been devoted to understanding the relationship between stressors and subjective age in older adults. We focus on the combined impact of major life-event stressors and daily stressors on multiple indicators of subjective age: felt age, ideal age, and look age. Furthermore, we examine the process by which daily stressors relate to subjective ages by testing whether positive affect, control, and negative affect mediate this relationship. Method Using a daily-diary design, the current study measured older adults' (60-96 years old) stressors, subjective ages, personal control, and affect. Results Felt, ideal, and look ages each demonstrated a unique pattern of interactions between daily stressors and major life-event stressors. Furthermore, our findings suggest that on the daily level, the relationship between stressors and felt age is mediated by negative affect but not by control and positive affect. Discussion Findings indicate the need to consider the broader contextual picture of stressors, as well as their differential impact on multiple indicators of subjective age.}, number={4}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Bellingtier, Jennifer A. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Kotter-Grühn, Dana}, year={2017}, month={Jul}, pages={613–621} } @article{monazah_bellingtier_gall_neupert_2016, title={Does ethnicity affect how older adults deal with stressors at home?}, volume={22}, url={https://scholar.utc.edu/mps/vol22/iss1/10}, number={1}, journal={Modern Psychological Studies}, author={Monazah, S.S.u and Bellingtier, J.A.g and Gall, A.A.g and Neupert, S.D.}, year={2016}, pages={90–96} } @article{begeny_whitehouse_methe_codding_stage_nuepert_2015, title={Do Intervention-embedded Assessment Procedures Successfully Measure Student Growth in Reading?}, volume={52}, ISSN={0033-3085}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pits.21843}, DOI={10.1002/pits.21843}, abstractNote={Effective intervention delivery requires ongoing assessment to determine whether students are learning at the desired rate. Intervention programs with embedded assessment procedures (i.e., assessment that occurs naturally during the process of delivering intervention) can potentially enhance instructional decisions. However, there is almost no psychometric research on this type of assessment procedure. This study was designed to examine the psychometric characteristics of three types of progress measures that are embedded within a commonly used reading intervention program. Results indicated that generalized gains across different oral reading fluency passages predict concurrent gains on common and comprehensive tests of reading fluency, and that immediate instructional gains measured during instruction were significantly different from zero and thus sensitive to intervention effects. Overall findings suggest that at least some embedded assessment procedures demonstrate predictive validity and that these types of procedures have the potential to assist educators with data-driven instructional decisions about students’ responsiveness to intervention.}, number={6}, journal={Psychology in the Schools}, publisher={Wiley}, author={Begeny, John C. and Whitehouse, Mary H. and Methe, Scott A. and Codding, Robin S. and Stage, Scott A. and Nuepert, Shevaun}, year={2015}, month={Jul}, pages={578–593} } @article{mroczek_stawski_turiano_chan_almeida_neupert_spiro_2015, title={Emotional Reactivity and Mortality: Longitudinal Findings From the VA Normative Aging Study}, volume={70}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbt107}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbt107}, abstractNote={OBJECTIVES Evidence suggests a predictive association between emotion and mortality risk. However, no study has examined dynamic aspects of emotion in relation to mortality. This study used an index of emotional reactivity, defined as changes in positive or negative affect in response to daily stressors, to predict 10-year survival. METHODS An 8-day daily diary study was conducted in 2002 on 181 men aged 58-88. Multilevel models were employed to estimate emotional reactivity coefficients, which were subsequently entered into a Cox proportional hazards model to predict mortality. RESULTS Results indicated that positive emotional reactivity, that is, greater decreases in positive affect in response to daily stressors, increased mortality risk. Negative emotional reactivity did not predict mortality. DISCUSSION Findings highlight the potential importance of dynamic aspects of positive affect in prediction of physical health outcomes such as mortality.}, number={3}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Mroczek, Daniel K. and Stawski, Robert S. and Turiano, Nicholas A. and Chan, Wai and Almeida, David M. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Spiro, Avron, III}, year={2015}, month={May}, pages={398–406} } @article{kotter-grühn_neupert_stephan_2015, title={Feeling old today? Daily health, stressors, and affect explain day-to-day variability in subjective age}, volume={30}, ISSN={0887-0446 1476-8321}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2015.1061130}, DOI={10.1080/08870446.2015.1061130}, abstractNote={Objectives: Subjective age is an important correlate of health, well-being, and longevity. So far, little is known about short-term variability in subjective age and the circumstances under which individuals feel younger/older in daily life. This study examined whether (a) older adults’ felt age fluctuates on a day-to-day basis, (b) daily changes in health, stressors, and affect explain fluctuations in felt age, and (c) the daily associations between felt age and health, stressors, or affect are time-ordered. Method: Using an eight-day daily diary approach, N = 43 adults (60–96 years, M = 74.65, SD = 8.19) filled out daily questionnaires assessing subjective age, health, daily stressors, and affect. Data were analysed using multilevel modelling. Main outcome measures: Subjective age, health, daily stressors, affect. Results: Intra-individual variability in felt age was not explained by time but by short-term variability in other variables. Specifically, on days when participants experienced more than average health problems, stress, or negative affect they felt older than on days with average health, stress, or negative affect. No time-ordered effects were found. Conclusion: Bad health, many stressors, and negative affective experiences constitute circumstances under which older adults feel older than they typically do. Thus, daily measures of subjective age could be markers of health and well-being.}, number={12}, journal={Psychology & Health}, publisher={Informa UK Limited}, author={Kotter-Grühn, Dana and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Stephan, Yannick}, year={2015}, month={Jul}, pages={1470–1485} } @article{neupert_ennis_ramsey_gall_2015, title={Solving Tomorrow’s Problems Today? Daily Anticipatory Coping and Reactivity to Daily Stressors}, volume={71}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbv003}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbv003}, abstractNote={OBJECTIVES The present study examined the day-to-day fluctuation of state-like anticipatory coping (coping employed prior to stressors) and how these coping processes relate to important outcomes for older adults (i.e., physical health, affect, memory failures). METHOD Forty-three older adults aged 60-96 (M = 74.65, SD = 8.19) participated in an 8-day daily diary study of anticipatory coping, stressors, health, affect, and memory failures. Participants reported anticipatory coping behaviors on one day with respect to 6 distinct stressor domains that could occur the following day. RESULTS Multilevel models indicated that anticipatory coping changes from day to day and within stressor domains. Lagged associations suggested that yesterday's anticipatory coping for potential upcoming arguments is related to today's physical health and affect. Increased stagnant deliberation is associated with reduced cognitive reactivity (i.e., fewer memory failures) to arguments the next day. DISCUSSION Taken together, these findings suggest that anticipatory coping is dynamic and associated with important daily outcomes.}, number={4}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Ennis, Gilda E. and Ramsey, Jennifer L. and Gall, Agnes A.}, year={2015}, month={Feb}, pages={650–660} } @article{ramsey_neupert_mroczek_spiro_2015, title={The effects of daily co-occurrence of affect on older adults’ reactivity to health stressors}, volume={31}, ISSN={0887-0446 1476-8321}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2015.1111368}, DOI={10.1080/08870446.2015.1111368}, abstractNote={Objectives: The present study examined age differences among older adults in the daily co-occurrence of affect and its potential role in buffering the negative effects of health stressors. Design: Participants were from the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study and included 249 young-old adults (age = 60–79 years, M = 71.6) and 64 old-old adults (age = 80–89, M = 82.9) who completed questionnaires assessing stressors, physical health symptoms, and positive and negative affect for eight consecutive days. Results: An independent samples t-test showed young-old and old-old adults did not significantly differ in their mean levels of daily co-occurrence of affect. The between-person relationships among stressors, health and daily co-occurrence of affect revealed that neither stressors nor health were significantly related to daily co-occurrence of affect. However, results from a multilevel model revealed a three-way cross-level interaction (health stressor × age group × co-occurrence of affect) where old-old adults with higher levels of co-occurrence of affect were less emotionally reactive to health stressors than young-old adults. Conclusion: These findings provide support for the assertion that co-occurrence of affect functions in an adaptive capacity and highlight the importance of examining domain-specific stressors.}, number={3}, journal={Psychology & Health}, publisher={Informa UK Limited}, author={Ramsey, Jennifer L. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Mroczek, Daniel K. and Spiro, Avron, III}, year={2015}, month={Dec}, pages={364–378} } @article{davis_usher_dearing_barkai_crowell-doom_neupert_mantzoros_crowell_2014, title={Attachment and the Metabolic Syndrome in Midlife}, volume={76}, ISSN={0033-3174}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000107}, DOI={10.1097/psy.0000000000000107}, abstractNote={Objective Adult attachment discourse patterns and current family relationship quality were examined as correlates of health behaviors and number of metabolic syndrome (MetS) criteria met, and as mediators of the link between childhood adversity and these health outcomes. Methods A sample of 215 white/European American and black/African American adults aged 35 to 55 years were examined using a cross-sectional study design. Discourse was assessed with the Adult Attachment Interview, using coherence (a marker of attachment security), unresolved trauma/loss (a marker of disorganized cognitions related to trauma or loss), and idealization (minimizing stressful experiences and their impact) scores. Relationship quality, adverse childhood experiences, and current depressive symptoms were assessed, as were health behaviors of diet, exercise, and smoking. MetS includes obesity, elevated blood pressure, elevated fasting glucose, high triglycerides, and low high-density lipoprotein cholesterol. Results Using path analysis and including childhood adversity severity and depressive symptoms in the model, both Adult Attachment Interview coherence and unresolved trauma/loss were directly linked to the number of MetS criteria (r = 0.186 and r = 0.170, respectively). Idealization was indirectly linked to MetS through poor diet (r = 0.183). The final model explained 21% of the variance in scores for the number of MetS criteria met. Conclusions Insecure adult attachment is associated with increased risk of MetS.}, number={8}, journal={Psychosomatic Medicine}, publisher={Ovid Technologies (Wolters Kluwer Health)}, author={Davis, Cynthia R. and Usher, Nicole and Dearing, Eric and Barkai, Ayelet R. and Crowell-Doom, Cynthia and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Mantzoros, Christos S. and Crowell, Judith A.}, year={2014}, month={Oct}, pages={611–621} } @article{whitfield_neupert_bruce_sims_morgan_thorpe_2014, title={Stress, longevity and cardiovascular outcomes among African American families in the Jackson Heart Study}, volume={24}, number={4}, journal={Ethnicity & Disease}, author={Whitfield, K.E. and Neupert, S.D. and Bruce, M.A. and Sims, M. and Morgan, A.A. and Thorpe, R.J.}, year={2014}, pages={456–461} } @misc{osborne_neupert_2013, title={A Brief Introduction to Hierarchical Linear Modeling}, ISBN={9789462094048}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-6209-404-8_9}, DOI={10.1007/978-94-6209-404-8_9}, abstractNote={Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM; also referred to as multilevel modeling or MLM) is becoming more common throughout all areas of the social sciences because of its flexibility and unique advantages not present in more traditional techniques (Osborne, 2000).}, journal={Handbook of Quantitative Methods for Educational Research}, publisher={SensePublishers}, author={Osborne, Jason W. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2013}, pages={187–198} } @misc{neupert_2013, title={Emotional Reactivity to Daily Stressors Using a Random Coefficients Model with SAS PROC MIXED: A Repeated Measures Analysis}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781483384450.n9}, DOI={10.4135/9781483384450.n9}, journal={Hierarchical Linear Modeling: Guide and Applications}, publisher={SAGE Publications, Inc.}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2013}, pages={205–218} } @article{haskett_neupert_okado_2013, title={Factors Associated with 3-Year Stability and Change in Parenting Behavior of Abusive Parents}, volume={23}, ISSN={1062-1024 1573-2843}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/S10826-013-9729-Y}, DOI={10.1007/S10826-013-9729-Y}, number={2}, journal={Journal of Child and Family Studies}, publisher={Springer Science and Business Media LLC}, author={Haskett, Mary E. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Okado, Yuko}, year={2013}, month={Feb}, pages={263–274} } @article{agrigoroaei_neupert_lachman_2013, title={Maintaining a Sense of Control in the Context of Cognitive Challenge}, volume={26}, ISSN={1662-9647 1662-971X}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/A000078}, DOI={10.1024/1662-9647/A000078}, abstractNote={ We considered the functional role of control beliefs for cognitive performance by focusing on patterns of stability across multiple trials increasing in level of difficulty. We assessed 56 adults aged 18–88 on working memory tasks. We examined stability vs. lability (intraindividual variability, IIV) in control beliefs and the relationships with anxiety, distraction, and performance. Age was positively associated with IIV in control and performance, and IIV increased with task difficulty. Those maintaining stable control beliefs had better performance and showed less anxiety and distraction. Those with lower stability and less control showed steeper declines in performance and increases in distraction. The findings suggest that stability of control beliefs may serve a protective function in the context of cognitively challenging tasks. }, number={1}, journal={GeroPsych}, publisher={Hogrefe Publishing Group}, author={Agrigoroaei, Stefan and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Lachman, Margie E.}, year={2013}, month={Jan}, pages={49–59} } @article{summers_neupert_2013, title={Social activity, negative affect, and argument severity: Their relationship as a function of age}, volume={23}, journal={Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences}, author={Summers, B.J.u and Neupert, S.D.}, year={2013}, pages={7–11} } @article{neupert_2013, title={Teaching tips for graduate methods classes}, journal={Adult Development and Aging Newsletter}, publisher={APA Division 20}, author={Neupert, S.D.}, year={2013} } @article{tonni_neupert_2012, title={Effects of neuroticism and gender on daily physical health symptoms}, journal={Ink: Undergraduate Research Journal}, author={Tonni, N.T.u and Neupert, S.D.}, year={2012} } @article{neupert_allaire_2012, title={I think I can, I think I can: Examining the within-person coupling of control beliefs and cognition in older adults.}, volume={27}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026447}, DOI={10.1037/a0026447}, abstractNote={We examined short-term intraindividual variability in control beliefs (competence and locus of control) and cognitive performance and the extent to which the constructs travel together over time. Thirty-six older adults (M = 74 years, SD = 5.51) completed questionnaires and cognitive tests twice each day for 60 consecutive days. Results indicated that control beliefs fluctuate within people across time. Multilevel models revealed that control and competence are coupled with concurrent and subsequent performance, but the benefit of occasion-level increases in control depends on individuals' average control. These findings underscore the importance of examining constructs using a within-person approach to identify dynamic processes in cognitive aging.}, number={3}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Allaire, Jason C.}, year={2012}, month={Sep}, pages={742–749} } @article{khaledi_neupert_2012, title={Nice guys finish last? The relationship of financial deterioration to personality based on gender}, journal={Ink: Undergraduate Research Journal}, author={Khaledi, A.D.u and Neupert, S.D.}, year={2012} } @article{neupert_patterson_davis_allaire_2011, title={Age Differences in Daily Predictors of Forgetting to Take Medication: The Importance of Context and Cognition}, volume={37}, ISSN={0361-073X 1096-4657}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2011.590757}, DOI={10.1080/0361073x.2011.590757}, abstractNote={The present study examined age differences in the within-person daily associations of basic cognition, everyday cognition, and busyness with forgetting to take medication. The authors extend previous interindividual difference findings by conducting a daily diary study of a baseline assessment and 8 consecutive days of 40 older adults (age = 60–89 years, M = 74.86) and 31 younger adults (age = 18–20 years, M = 18.30) where basic cognition, everyday cognition, busyness, and forgetting medication were assessed each day and entered simultaneously into one model. Results from a logistic multilevel model indicated that performance on Letter Series was beneficial for both age groups, but the role of fluctuations in busyness on forgetting to take medications was opposite for younger and older adults. Younger adults remembered to take their medication the most on days when they had high everyday cognition and were busier. Older adults remembered to take their medication the most on days when they had high everyday cognition but were less busy. These findings highlight the importance of contextual variation in busyness in relation to daily medication adherence for younger and older adults.}, number={4}, journal={Experimental Aging Research}, publisher={Informa UK Limited}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Patterson, Taryn R. and Davis, Agnes A. and Allaire, Jason C.}, year={2011}, month={Jul}, pages={435–448} } @article{hess_emery_neupert_2011, title={Longitudinal Relationships Between Resources, Motivation, and Functioning}, volume={67B}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr100}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/gbr100}, abstractNote={OBJECTIVES We investigated how fluctuations and linear changes in health and cognitive resources influence the motivation to engage in complex cognitive activity and the extent to which motivation mediated the relationship between changing resources and cognitively demanding activities. METHOD Longitudinal data from 332 adults aged 20-85 years were examined. Motivation was assessed using a composite of Need for Cognition and Personal Need for Structure and additional measures of health, sensory functioning, cognitive ability, and self-reported activity engagement. RESULTS Multilevel modeling revealed that age-typical changes in health, sensory functions, and ability were associated with changes in motivation, with the impact of declining health on motivation being particularly strong in older adulthood. Changes in motivation, in turn, predicted involvement in cognitive and social activities as well as changes in cognitive ability. Finally, motivation was observed to partially mediate the relationship between changes in resources and cognitively demanding activities. DISCUSSION Our results suggest that motivation may play an important role in determining the course of cognitive change and involvement in cognitively demanding everyday activities in adulthood.}, number={3}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Hess, T. M. and Emery, L. and Neupert, S. D.}, year={2011}, month={Sep}, pages={299–308} } @inbook{lachman_neupert_agrigoroaei_2011, place={Paris}, title={The Relevance of Control Beliefs for Health and Aging}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-380882-0.00011-5}, DOI={10.1016/b978-0-12-380882-0.00011-5}, abstractNote={Adults and those in later life with a high sense of control appear better off on many indicators of health and well-being. However, those who have a lower sense of control may be at increased risk for a wide range of negative behavioral, affective, and functional outcomes, including higher levels of depression, anxiety, and stress, use of fewer health protective behaviors and compensatory memory strategies (internal or external memory aids), and have poorer health and memory functioning. The apparent decline of the sense of control associated with aging is of concern especially given the adaptive value of maintaining beliefs in one's control over outcomes. This chapter presents a wealth of information about control beliefs, but there is much to explore before to fully understand the dynamic processes involved in changes and the linkages with outcomes. Sense of control is a promising dimension because it is amenable to change unlike more traditional stable personality traits. This can potentially lead researchers in the direction of new interventions to promote optimal aging.}, booktitle={Handbook of the Psychology of Aging}, publisher={Academic Press}, author={Lachman, Margie E. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Agrigoroaei, Stefan}, editor={Schaie, K.W. and Willis, S.L.Editors}, year={2011}, pages={175–190} } @article{ray_neupert_2010, title={Association of stress level and physical activity on the number of psychologically influenced illnesses experienced by young and older adults}, volume={7}, journal={Ink: Undergraduate Research Journal}, author={Ray, J.N.u and Neupert, S.D.}, year={2010}, pages={42–50} } @article{mallers_charles_neupert_almeida_2010, title={Perceptions of childhood relationships with mother and father: Daily emotional and stressor experiences in adulthood.}, volume={46}, ISSN={1939-0599 0012-1649}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021020}, DOI={10.1037/a0021020}, abstractNote={Adults who report having had high-quality relationships with their parents during childhood have better overall mental health and are at decreased risk for mental disorders compared with those who report low parental relationship quality. Researchers have predominantly focused on the relationship with the mother, often times excluding the unique role that fathers may play in the long-term development of their offspring. The current study examined the unique associations of recalled childhood experiences of mother-child and father-child relationship quality with daily emotional experiences and stress processes in adulthood. Men and women (N = 912, ages 25-74) retrospectively reported the quality of their childhood relationships with their mother and father. Later, they reported their daily psychological distress and stressor exposure every night over 8 consecutive evenings. Results indicate that mother-child relationship quality was related to lower levels of daily psychological distress. The quality of both mother-child and father-child relationships was related to stressor exposure, but only father-son relationship quality was related to lower levels of emotional reactivity to stressors during adulthood.}, number={6}, journal={Developmental Psychology}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Mallers, Melanie H. and Charles, Susan T. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Almeida, David M.}, year={2010}, pages={1651–1661} } @article{mcdonald-miszczak_neupert_gutman_2009, title={Does Cognitive Ability Explain Inaccuracy in Older Adults' Self-Reported Medication Adherence?}, volume={28}, ISSN={["1552-4523"]}, DOI={10.1177/0733464808330821}, abstractNote={ This study examines the accuracy of younger-old and older-old adults’ self-reported adherence over a 3-month period and the potential interactive relationship between self-report accuracy and cognitive abilities. For 3 months, 71 younger-old ( M = 68.10, range = 57 to 74) and 62 older-old ( M = 80.31, range = 75 to 89) adults had their actual and self-reported adherence monitored. Cognitive tests assessing episodic and prospective memory were given at the beginning of the study. Multilevel models indicate that 32% of the variability in objective adherence was from between-person differences whereas 68% was from within-person fluctuations. There were age differences in the coupling of actual and self-reported adherence over time, such that younger-old adults’ self-reports less accurately reflected their actual adherence. Subsequent models indicate that age differences in the coupled relationship were further moderated by cognitive abilities. Results suggest that the relationships among age, cognitive abilities, and accuracy of self-reported adherence are far from simple. }, number={5}, journal={JOURNAL OF APPLIED GERONTOLOGY}, author={McDonald-Miszczak, Leslie and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Gutman, Gloria}, year={2009}, month={Oct}, pages={560–581} } @article{neupert_lachman_whitbourne_2009, title={Exercise Self-Efficacy and Control Beliefs: Effects on Exercise Behavior After an Exercise Intervention for Older Adults}, volume={17}, ISSN={["1063-8652"]}, DOI={10.1123/japa.17.1.1}, abstractNote={The current study examined exercise self-efficacy and exercise behavior during and after a strength-training intervention program with older adults. A model with cross-lagged and contemporaneous paths was tested with structural equations. Within testing occasions, higher physical resistance was related to greater beliefs in efficacy and control over exercise. At 3 months into the intervention, those who had higher physical resistance were less likely to show subsequent changes in beliefs. Those who had higher self-efficacy and control beliefs at 6 months were more likely to report that they were still exercising at 9 and 12 months after the intervention. Findings indicate that exercise self-efficacy and exercise behavior are associated with one another and that beliefs developed during an intervention are important for maintenance of an exercise regimen.}, number={1}, journal={JOURNAL OF AGING AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Lachman, Margie E. and Whitbourne, Stacey B.}, year={2009}, month={Jan}, pages={1–16} } @article{rojas_neupert_2009, title={I think I can, I think I can: Control beliefs matter for stressor resolution}, volume={5}, journal={Undergraduate Research Journal}, author={Rojas, V.A.u and Neupert, S.D.}, year={2009}, pages={49–52} } @article{heflin_neupert_2009, title={Mastery and daily stressors influence daily control over memory}, volume={5}, journal={Undergraduate Research Journal}, author={Heflin, M.A.u and Neupert, S.D.}, year={2009}, pages={84–87} } @article{isaacowitz_toner_neupert_2009, title={Use of gaze for real-time mood regulation: Effects of age and attentional functioning.}, volume={24}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017706}, DOI={10.1037/a0017706}, abstractNote={Older adults show positive preferences in their gaze toward emotional faces, and such preferences appear to be activated when older adults are in bad moods. This suggests that age-related gaze preferences serve a mood regulatory role, but whether they actually function to improve mood over time has yet to be tested. We investigated links between fixation and mood change in younger and older adults, as well as the moderating role of attentional functioning. AgexFixationxAttentional Functioning interactions emerged such that older adults with better executive functioning were able to resist mood declines by showing positive gaze preferences. Implications for the function of age-related positive gaze preferences are discussed.}, number={4}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Isaacowitz, Derek M. and Toner, Kaitlin and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2009}, pages={989–994} } @inbook{almeida_charles_neupert_2008, place={New York}, title={Assessing health behaviors across individuals, situations, and time}, booktitle={Social Structures and Aging}, publisher={Springer}, author={Almeida, D.M. and Charles, S.T. and Neupert, S.D.}, editor={Schaie, K.W. and Abeles, R.P.Editors}, year={2008}, pages={97–111} } @misc{neupert_stawski_almeida_2008, title={Considerations for Sampling Time in Research on Aging: Examples From Research on Stress and Cognition}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781412976589.n29}, DOI={10.4135/9781412976589.n29}, journal={Handbook of Cognitive Aging: Interdisciplinary Perspectives}, publisher={SAGE Publications, Inc.}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Stawski, Robert S. and Almeida, David M.}, year={2008}, pages={492–505} } @article{whitbourne_neupert_lachman_2008, title={Daily physical activity: Relation to everyday memory in adulthood}, volume={27}, ISSN={["1552-4523"]}, DOI={10.1177/0733464807312175}, abstractNote={ This article examines the relationship between daily physical activity and everyday memory using an 8-day diary design with young, middle-aged, and older adults. Contrary to expectations, age differences were not reported in the frequency of memory failures and daily physical activity at the between-person level. Multilevel modeling, however, indicated that on days when adults engaged in leisure exercise (physical activity performed during leisure or free time), they reported fewer memory failures, and this was most apparent for older adults. Lagged analyses indicated that when leisure activity was reported on one day, fewer memory failures were reported the next day, and this was especially true for older adults. Thus, findings demonstrate that the benefits of physical activity for memory in later life are observable on a short-term daily basis. }, number={3}, journal={JOURNAL OF APPLIED GERONTOLOGY}, author={Whitbourne, Stacey B. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Lachman, Margie E.}, year={2008}, month={Jun}, pages={331–349} } @article{neupert_mroczek_spiro_2008, title={Neuroticism moderates the daily relation between stressors and memory failures.}, volume={23}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.287}, DOI={10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.287}, abstractNote={The present investigation extends previous work on the relationship between daily stressors and memory failures in a naturalistic setting by examining whether this relationship varies across levels of neuroticism. A daily diary study of 333 older adults (mean age = 73.27 years, SD = 7.17) in the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study (see A. Spiro & R. Bossé, 2001, for additional information) was used to examine whether there were neuroticism differences in cognitive reactivity to daily stressors. Multilevel models indicated that on days when people high in neuroticism experienced stressors, particularly interpersonal stressors, they were more likely to report memory failures compared to those who were lower in neuroticism. The findings may have important implications for age-related cognitive decline.}, number={2}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Mroczek, Daniel K. and Spiro, Avron}, year={2008}, month={Jun}, pages={287–296} } @article{neupert_almeida_charles_2007, title={Age Differences in Reactivity to Daily Stressors: The Role of Personal Control}, volume={62}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/62.4.P216}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/62.4.P216}, abstractNote={We examined age and control belief differences in physical and emotional reactivity to daily stressors in four domains: interpersonal, work, network, and home. We combined data from the National Study of Daily Experiences and the Midlife in the United States survey, resulting in 1,031 participants who reported on 7,229 days. Findings from multilevel models suggest that age and control beliefs play an important role in a person's reactivity to interpersonal, network, and work stressors. Specifically, older age and lower perceived constraints were each related to lower emotional and physical reactivity to interpersonal stressors. High mastery buffered the physical effects of work stressors for younger and older adults, and high mastery was important for middle-aged adults' emotional reactivity to network stressors. High constraint was associated with the strongest physical reactivity to network stressors for younger and older adults.}, number={4}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Neupert, S. D. and Almeida, D. M. and Charles, S. T.}, year={2007}, month={Jul}, pages={P216–P225} } @article{neupert_2007, title={Aging has its Ups and Downs, but it’s Always Interesting}, volume={42}, url={https://www.apadivisions.org/division-1/publications/newsletters/general/2007/10-issue.pdf}, number={2}, journal={The General Psychologist}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2007}, pages={29–30} } @inbook{neupert_2007, title={Daily stressors and memory failures}, volume={12}, booktitle={Research and Practice in Alzheimer’s Disease and Cognitive Decline}, author={Neupert, S.D.}, editor={Vellas, B. and Grundman and Feldman, H. and Fitten, L.J. and Winblad, B. and Giacobini, E.Editors}, year={2007}, pages={144–149} } @article{yorgason_almeida_neupert_spiro_hoffman_2006, title={A dyadic examination of daily health symptoms and emotional well-being in late-life couples}, volume={55}, ISSN={["1741-3729"]}, DOI={10.1111/j.1741-3729.2006.00430.x}, abstractNote={Abstract: This study investigated the link between daily health symptoms and spousal emotional well‐being in a sample of 96 older dyads. Higher negative mood and lower positive mood were associated with spousal symptoms in couples wherein husbands or wives reported higher average levels of symptoms. For wives, partner effects were moderated by husbands’ marital satisfaction and illness severity. Specifically, higher husband marital satisfaction and illness severity were associated with higher negative mood and lower positive mood for wives on days where husbands reported higher symptom levels. In their work with later‐life families, practitioners and educators should address long‐term and daily health‐related relationship stressors.}, number={5}, journal={FAMILY RELATIONS}, author={Yorgason, Jeremy B. and Almeida, David and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Spiro, Avron, III and Hoffman, Lesa}, year={2006}, month={Dec}, pages={613–624} } @article{neupert_almeida_mroczek_spiro_2006, title={Daily stressors and memory failures in a naturalistic setting: Findings from the va normative aging study.}, volume={21}, ISSN={1939-1498 0882-7974}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.424}, DOI={10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.424}, abstractNote={Laboratory studies of stress and memory have generally found that people with more stress tend to have poorer cognitive performance. The present investigation examined the relationship between stressors and memory failures in a naturalistic setting via a daily diary study of 333 older adults in the VA Normative Aging Study. Multilevel models indicated that on days when people experienced stressors, particularly interpersonal stressors, they were more likely to report memory failures. These stressors were also associated with an increase in memory failures from one day to the next. The findings may be important for preventions to mitigate age-related cognitive decline.}, number={2}, journal={Psychology and Aging}, publisher={American Psychological Association (APA)}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Almeida, David M. and Mroczek, Daniel K. and Spiro, Avron}, year={2006}, pages={424–429} } @inbook{neupert_2006, place={Boston}, edition={10th ed.}, title={Lifespan Development Timeline}, booktitle={Life-Span Development}, publisher={McGraw-Hill}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D.}, editor={Santrock, John W.Editor}, year={2006} } @article{neupert_miller_lachman_2006, title={Physiological Reactivity to Cognitive Stressors: Variations by Age and Socioeconomic Status}, volume={62}, ISSN={0091-4150 1541-3535}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/17DU-21AA-5HUK-7UFG}, DOI={10.2190/17DU-21AA-5HUK-7UFG}, abstractNote={ The present study focused on age and SES differences in stress reactivity in response to cognitively challenging tasks. Specifically, we assessed within-person trajectories of cortisol, a steroid hormone released by the adrenal gland in response to stressors, before, during, and after exposure to cognitively challenging tasks. We extend the current literature by simultaneously examining age and SES differences in physiological reactivity. Findings suggest that age and SES both play an important role in reactivity, such that it was the older adults with higher SES who were the most physiologically reactive to cognitive stressors. Implications of these findings for cognitive aging research are discussed. }, number={3}, journal={The International Journal of Aging and Human Development}, publisher={SAGE Publications}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and Miller, Lisa M. Soederberg and Lachman, Margie E.}, year={2006}, month={Apr}, pages={221–235} } @inbook{mroczek_shapiro_griffin_neupert_2006, place={New York, NY}, title={Societal Influences on Adult Personality, Self-Regulation, and Health}, booktitle={Social Structures, Aging, and Self-Regulation in the Elderly}, publisher={Springer}, author={Mroczek, Daniel K. and Shapiro, Avron, III and Griffin, Paul W. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, editor={Schaie, K. Warner and Carstensen, Laura L.Editors}, year={2006}, pages={69–83} } @article{lachman_neupert_bertrand_jette_2006, title={The effects of strength training on memory in older adults}, volume={14}, ISSN={["1543-267X"]}, DOI={10.1123/japa.14.1.59}, abstractNote={The authors examined whether resistance training has an effect on working memory span. Participants included 210 community-residing older adults with at least one disability from the Strong for Life program, a randomized controlled trial that examined the effects of home-based resistance exercise. Memory was assessed with the WAIS backward digit span at baseline and 3 and 6 months into the intervention. Although there were no differences between the experimental treatment and control groups in average levels of memory change, within the treatment group change in resistance level during the intervention was a significant predictor of memory change, controlling for age, education, sex, and disability level. The results suggest that strength training can benefit memory among older adults, especially when using higher resistance levels.}, number={1}, journal={JOURNAL OF AGING AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY}, author={Lachman, ME and Neupert, SD and Bertrand, R and Jette, AM}, year={2006}, month={Jan}, pages={59–73} } @article{neupert_almeida_mroczek_spiro_2006, title={The effects of the Columbia shuttle disaster on the daily lives of older adults: Findings from the VA Normative Aging Study}, volume={10}, ISSN={["1364-6915"]}, DOI={10.1080/13607860500409682}, abstractNote={During 2002–2003 the VA Normative Aging Study conducted an eight-day diary survey of stressors and well-being. A sub-sample of 19 men and 13 women (mean age = 71.78) completed daily questionnaires before and after the Columbia shuttle exploded on 1st February 2003, presenting a unique look into peoples’ daily lives before and after a tragic event. Results indicated no significant changes in negative affect or physical symptoms, but people reported significant decreases in both positive affect and memory failures on days following the shuttle explosion. Implications of these findings and directions for future research are discussed.}, number={3}, journal={AGING & MENTAL HEALTH}, author={Neupert, S. D. and Almeida, D. M. and Mroczek, D. K. and Spiro, A., III}, year={2006}, month={May}, pages={272–281} } @article{almeida_neupert_banks_serido_2005, title={Do Daily Stress Processes Account for Socioeconomic Health Disparities?}, volume={60}, ISSN={1079-5014 1758-5368}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/60.special_issue_2.s34}, DOI={10.1093/geronb/60.special_issue_2.s34}, abstractNote={Objectives. The present study examined the extent to which daily stressor severity and appraisals of the stressors accounted for socioeconomic disparities in health. Methods. Data from the National Study of Daily Experiences and the Midlife in the United States Survey were combined for the current analyses, resulting in 1,031 respondents who reported on 7,229 days. Results. Respondents without a high school degree experienced more severe stressors and appraised stressors as posing greater risk to their financial situation and to their self-concept than respondents with a high school or college degree. Differences in severity and stressor appraisal accounted for education differences in psychological distress and physical health symptoms. Discussion. Findings suggest the importance of considering variation across stressors, particularly implications for self-concept, in understanding sources of differential stressor vulnerability.}, number={Special_Issue_2}, journal={The Journals of Gerontology: Series B}, publisher={Oxford University Press (OUP)}, author={Almeida, David M. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Banks, Sean R. and Serido, Joyce}, year={2005}, month={Oct}, pages={S34–S39} } @inbook{neupert_lachman_2005, place={Thousand Oaks, CA}, title={Life Events}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781412950565.n253}, DOI={10.4135/9781412950565.n253}, booktitle={Encyclopedia of Applied Developmental Science}, publisher={SAGE Publications, Inc.}, author={Neupert, S.D. and Lachman, M.E.}, editor={Fisher, C.B. and Lerner, R.M.Editors}, year={2005}, pages={658–661} } @article{horn mallers_almeida_neupert_2005, title={Women's daily physical health symptoms and stressful experiences across adulthood}, volume={20}, ISSN={0887-0446 1476-8321}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08870440512331317698}, DOI={10.1080/08870440512331317698}, abstractNote={This study investigated the extent to which the experience of daily stressors was related to women's age and daily health symptomology, such as flu and cold symptoms. Respondents were 562 women (aged 25–74) who were a part of the National Study of Daily Experiences (NSDE), a telephone diary study examining daily stressful events. The respondents were interviewed by telephone on eight consecutive nights, which resulted in a total of 3978 days of information analysed. Overall, women had at least one physical symptom on 59% of the study days and at least one daily stressor on 40% of the study days. Results from a series of ANOVAs showed that young and middle-aged women reported more frequent physical symptoms than did the older women. This age pattern was similar to the incidence of daily stressful experiences. Furthermore, daily stressors, specifically interpersonal tensions, were shown to significantly mediate the age–symptom relationship. These findings suggest that women who are exposed to events that threaten their interpersonal relationships are at a greater risk for symptoms of ill-health.}, number={3}, journal={Psychology & Health}, publisher={Informa UK Limited}, author={Horn Mallers, Melanie and Almeida, David M. and Neupert, Shevaun D.}, year={2005}, month={Jun}, pages={389–403} } @article{mcdonald-miszczak_2005, title={Younger-old and older-old adults' recall of medication instructions}, volume={24}, ISSN={["0714-9808"]}, DOI={10.1353/cja.2006.0013}, abstractNote={ABSTRACT}, number={4}, journal={CANADIAN JOURNAL ON AGING-REVUE CANADIENNE DU VIEILLISSEMENT}, author={McDonald-Miszczak, L}, year={2005}, pages={409–417} } @article{grzywacz_almeida_neupert_ettner_2004, title={Socioeconomic Status and Health: A Micro-level Analysis o Exposure and Vulnerability to Daily Stressors}, volume={45}, ISSN={0022-1465 2150-6000}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002214650404500101}, DOI={10.1177/002214650404500101}, abstractNote={This study examines the interconnections among education—as a proxy for socioeconomic status—stress, and physical and mental health by specifying differential exposure and vulnerability models using data from The National Study of Daily Experiences (N = 1,031). These daily diary data allowed assessment of the social distribution of a qualitatively different type of stressor than has previously been examined in sociological stress research—daily stressors, or hassles. Moreover, these data allowed a less biased assessment of stress exposure and a more micro-level examination of the connections between stress and health by socioeconomic status. Consistent with the broad literature describing socioeconomic inequalities in physical and mental health, the results of this study indicated that, on any given day, better-educated adults reported fewer physical symptoms and less psychological distress. Although better educated individuals reported more daily stressors, stressors reported by those with less education were more severe. Finally, neither exposure nor vulnerability explained socioeconomic differentials in daily health, but the results clearly indicate that the stressor-health association cannot be considered independent of socioeconomic status.}, number={1}, journal={Journal of Health and Social Behavior}, publisher={SAGE Publications}, author={Grzywacz, Joseph G. and Almeida, David M. and Neupert, Shevaun D. and Ettner, Susan L.}, year={2004}, month={Mar}, pages={1–16} } @article{neupert_mcdonald-miszczak_2004, title={Younger and Older Adults' Delayed Recall of Medication Instructions: The Role of Cognitive and Metacognitive Predictors}, volume={11}, ISSN={1382-5585 1744-4128}, url={http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825580490521403}, DOI={10.1080/13825580490521403}, abstractNote={The two goals of the present study were to replicate the results of Gould, McDonald-Miszczak, and Gregory (1999) and to examine cognitive and metacognitive variables as predictors of younger and older adults' delayed recall of cued medication instructions. Gould et al. (1999) examined younger and older adults' predictive accuracy for medications instructions using a similar methodology to the present study, but cognitive variables were not the focus of their study. Therefore, 105 younger adults (M = 19.90 years, SD = 1.43) and 58 older adults (M = 74.19 years, SD = 8.65) completed cognitive tests and reported their everyday and task-specific memory self-efficacy in the present study. Like the results of Gould et al. (1999), both age groups overestimated the memorability of the medication instructions in general and no age differences in predictive accuracy were found. Hierarchical regression results revealed that cognitive abilities and metacognitive beliefs played roles in younger and older adults' delayed recall of medication instructions, but the specific predictors differed. Recall was predicted by vocabulary ability and likelihood of recall ratings across age groups, but working memory ability was especially important for older adults' recall. Such differences might provide important insights into age-sensitive intervention strategies to improve adherence to prescription medications.}, number={4}, journal={Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition}, publisher={Informa UK Limited}, author={Neupert, Shevaun D. and McDonald-Miszczak, Leslie}, year={2004}, month={Dec}, pages={428–442} }